PodClips Logo
PodClips Logo
Huberman Lab
The Science of Love, Desire and Attachment
The Science of Love, Desire and Attachment

The Science of Love, Desire and Attachment

Huberman LabGo to Podcast Page

Andrew Huberman
·
37 Clips
·
Feb 14, 2022
Listen to Clips & Top Moments
Episode Summary
Episode Transcript
0:00
Welcome to the huberman Lab podcast, where we discuss science and science based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford school of medicine.
0:14
Today. We are going to talk about the psychology and the biology of
0:18
Desire. Love and attachment today happens to be Valentine's Day 2022.
0:24
However, the themes we are going to discuss pertain to desire love and attachment on any
0:30
Day. And indeed the mechanisms. We are going to discuss almost certainly. We're at play thousands of years ago, hundreds of years ago and no doubt will still be at play in our minds and in our bodies and in our psychologies, for the decades, centuries and thousands of years to come
0:45
indeed today. I want to focus on core mechanisms that lead individuals to seek out, other
0:51
individuals with whom to mate, with with whom to have children with or not with whom to enter short or long-term
0:59
relationships.
1:00
With and perhaps to end
1:02
those relationships or to seek relationships on the side. So called infidelity. I'm certainly not going to encourage or
1:07
discourage any of these behaviors. I'm simply going to cover the
1:11
peer-reviewed scientific data on all these aspects of Desire, love and attachment. I'm going to discuss how our childhood attachment Styles as they're called influence. Our adult attachment Styles. Yes. You heard that right?
1:26
How we
1:27
attached or did not attach to
1:29
primary?
1:30
First in our childhood has much to do with
1:34
how we attach or fail to
1:35
attach to romantic Partners as adults because
1:39
the same neural circuits
1:41
the neurons and their connections in the brain and body
1:44
that underlie attachment between infant and
1:46
caregiver between toddler and parent or other caregiver and during adolescence and in our teenage years are repurposed for adult romantic attachments. I know that might be a little Eerie to think about but indeed that
2:00
That is true. Now. The fortunate thing is that regardless of our childhood attachment Styles and
2:05
experiences, the neural circuits for desire
2:08
love and attachment are quite plastic. They are amenable to change in response to both what
2:14
we think and what we
2:16
feel as well as what we
2:17
do. However,
2:19
all three aspects that were discussing today, desire, love and attachment are also strongly biologically driven. We're going to talk about biological mechanisms such as
2:30
Hormones biological mechanisms such as neurochemicals things
2:33
like dopamine, oxytocin and serotonin
2:36
and neural circuits brain areas and indeed areas of the body that interact with the brain that control. Whether or not we desire somebody or not, whether or not we lose or increase. Our desire for somebody over time whether or not we fall in love what love means. And whether or not the relationships, we form continued to include the
2:56
elements of desire and love over time or not in order.
3:00
Her
3:00
to illustrate. Just how powerfully our biology can shape our
3:02
perception of the attractiveness of other people. I want to
3:05
share with you the results of a couple of studies. Both studies, explore how people rate other people's attractiveness and in both studies. The major variable is that women are at different stages of their menstrual cycle. Now in the first study men are rating, the attractiveness of women according to the smell of those women.
3:25
Now, they're not smelling them directly. They're
3:27
smelling clothing that women wore for a couple of days.
3:30
Is at different phases
3:31
of their menstrual cycle. And
3:33
what they find is that men will rate the odors of women
3:36
as most attractive.
3:37
If those women wore those shirts
3:39
that clothing in the pre ovulatory phase of their cycle. Okay. So this is not to say that men do not find women attractive at other stages of their cycle.
3:49
It is to say that men find women's odors particularly attractive. If those odors were worn by women that are in the pre ovulatory,
3:58
phase of their
4:00
Menstrual cycle.
4:01
Okay. Now there was also a study that was done where women at different stages of their. Menstrual cycle are rating the odors of men and a similar. But mirror symmetric result was found such that women who are in the pre ovulatory phase of their menstrual cycle will rate men's odors as more
4:20
attractive than at other stages of their cycle.
4:23
So the simple way to put this is that there seems to be something special about the preovulatory phase of a woman's menstrual cycle.
4:30
That makes men rate them as more attractive
4:32
during that time and
4:33
women rate men
4:35
as more attractive during that particular time as well. So this is a bi-directional affect
4:40
the way that the second study was done where women are rating. Men was not just to smell the odors of
4:47
those men on T-shirts. They did that but
4:50
they correlated that with whether or not the shirts were worn by men that were particularly physically symmetrical that she had these men divided into groups. It was more of a Continuum rather.
5:00
Either rated according to body symmetry and face symmetry and women preferred more symmetrical men when they were doing the preference test during the pre ovulatory phase of their cycle. So again, the point is that that preovulatory phase of the cycle seems to create a
5:16
bi-directional mutual attractiveness.
5:19
Now, also extremely interesting is that this effect does really seem to have something to do with
5:24
ovulation, because in both
5:26
studies, they had women that were taking oral contraception or
5:30
Art and what they found. Was, if a woman is
5:32
taking oral contraception, it
5:34
prevented that peak in perceived
5:36
attractiveness by the men,
5:38
meaning men no longer perceived a woman to be more attractive at a particular phase of their cycle and also women taking oral contraception no longer prefer the odors of more symmetrical men during the pre ovulatory phase of their cycle. I want to make sure that it's especially clear that it is not the case that oral.
6:00
Option reduced the perception of a woman as
6:02
attractive that did not happen in these studies. It reduced
6:06
the further increase in a
6:08
male's perception of her as attractive
6:10
and if women took oral contraception, it prevented them from preferring more symmetrical, men, based on the odors of those men. Now, I realize there are a lot of variables here. We've got odors we've got symmetry. We've got menstrual cycle, preovulatory non preovulatory and we have whether or not people are taking contraception or not, but the
6:30
Basic finding is that depending on where women are in their menstrual cycle influences? Both men's perception of them as attractive and their perception of men as attractive and oral contraception eliminates that effect. So I share with you those data to illustrate that we often think that somebody is attractive or
6:48
not based on how they look their skin, their Hair,
6:53
Etc. But it also illustrates that their odor is a powerful cue for some people more than others, you know.
7:00
Some of us tend to be more
7:00
olfactory driven than others.
7:02
Although, if you watched the huberman Lab
7:03
podcast, episode that I did with Professor David Buss from the University of Texas, Austin, who's
7:09
a luminary in the field of evolutionary
7:11
psychology and has studied mate choice and mate, selection bias over decades. He's really one of the founders of that field. He emphasized findings that odor for many people is a maker or a deal-breaker
7:26
meaning. There are some people that even if somebody
7:29
has all the
7:30
Characteristics that they're looking for in terms of kindness and attractiveness and values and other features that would and should be a very high priority in
7:38
selecting a mate that if they, if someone does not like the way that person smells their innate body, odor, independent of colognes, and perfumes and soaps, Etc. Is that that's
7:49
often a complete and total deal-breaker. I'm sure there's some of you that can relate to that, and there's some of you perhaps for, which that is not the case. And you can't even imagine that being such a powerful variable and
8:00
Yet, the data suggest that indeed, it is a powerful variable for many people out there. Before. We begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
8:10
It is however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to Consumer information about science
8:15
and science related tools to the general public in
8:17
keeping with that theme. I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is thesis thesis makes custom nootropics. Now nootropics is not a word
8:27
that I'm usually a fan of because nootropic
8:30
Means smart drug, and there are a lot of different aspects to being smart or to intelligence. There's the ability to focus. There's the ability to tasks, which there's the ability to be
8:39
creative and each of those different forms of
8:42
intelligence or expressions of intelligence
8:44
involves different, brain, circuits, different neural, chemicals, and indeed different hormones in the body. So, I think it's rather naive to think that there could be
8:52
one smart drug, or nootropic thesis understands this. And so they've developed, custom nootropics that are tailored to the specific.
8:59
Vic goals that you might have, in terms of cognitive work or physical exercise, or
9:04
skill learning of any kind, and they tailor them
9:07
to the individual. They only use the
9:09
highest quality ingredients, many of,
9:11
which I've talked about here on the podcast. Things like Alpha GPC, which I personally use things like, phosphatidyl serine, which I also use on occasion.
9:19
However, they've taken different combinations of those
9:21
ingredients and put them into different
9:23
formulations design for particular goals or
9:26
endpoints. For instance. I have thesis
9:29
nootropics that
9:30
For motivation, others that are for clarity for mental Clarity, others, that I take
9:36
pre-workout, others that I take post-workout and so on and so forth.
9:40
Now, in addition to that thesis understands that, not every ingredient is terrific for everybody. In fact, one particular
9:47
substance. I've talked about on the
9:48
podcast, ginkgo biloba is very useful for a lot of people. However, I don't tolerate it. Well, I get vicious headaches from ginkgo biloba. So none of the formulations that they've made for me include Ginkgo.
9:59
Biloba. So again, these are custom nootropics that are tailored for your particular goals and where the ingredients really match. Your particular needs and don't include
10:08
things that aren't going to work for you or that create things like
10:11
headaches. So the personalization and the targeted
10:14
effects of the nootropics is really where the power comes from.
10:17
I've been using thesis nootropics for close to six months now
10:20
and I can confidently say that their nootropics have been a total game changer for me.
10:24
My go-to formula is the motivation formula if
10:28
I'm going to be training or working out.
10:29
Do I need extra energy and I use the clarity formula nowadays for any kind of writing or creative work. Those are the two that I've mainly been taking lately. If
10:38
you want to try your own personalized, nootropic starter kit. You can go online to take thesis.com huberman, you'll take a three-minute quiz and thesis will send you four different formulas to try in your first month. You will have the ability to try those different blends across that month, discover, which nootropics work best for you.
10:55
Your unique brain, chemistry and genetics. You can have a consult with them and then
10:59
They will give you the best nootropics for you.
11:02
Again, that's take thesis.com huberman and use the code huberman at checkout to get 10% off. Your first box. Today's episode is also brought To Us by athletic
11:11
greens now called a G1. I've been taking a G1 since 2012. So I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast reason. I started taking a G1 and the reason I still take a G1 once or twice a day, is that it covers all of my vitamin mineral probiotic needs probiotics are essential because they support what's called a healthy gut.
11:29
Crow, biome the gut microbiome is vital for things like metabolism hormone function. And also we now know our brain function, things like focus and memory and our general immune system, with a G1, I get the probiotics. I need, I get the vitamins and minerals that I need to cover. Any nutritional gaps, if I'm not eating
11:46
optimally and even if I
11:47
am eating, optimally a G1 can further support metabolism hormone function, etcetera. In
11:52
fact, whenever people ask me what's the One
11:55
supplement that I should take? If I can only take one supplement. I always say a g1i.
11:59
I take
12:00
mine early in the day. I mix it with water and some lemon or lime juice. I love
12:03
the way it tastes, and I'll take it again later in the day, typically in the late afternoon. If
12:07
you'd like to try out, let it greens. You can go to athletic greens.com huberman, and claim a special offer. They're giving you five free travel
12:15
packs. That make it really easy to mix up a G1 while you're on the road and the car on the plane
12:18
etcetera and a year supply of vitamin D3 K to there's a ton of data now supporting the fact that vitamin D3 is critical and that
12:27
most of us don't get enough vitamin D3.
12:29
If we're getting ample sunlight, Vitamin D3 is important for metabolism hormone function, brain function, and many other aspects of our
12:36
biology. So again, if you go to athletic greens.com huberman, you get a special offer of the athletic greens. Five free travel packs and the year supply of
12:44
vitamin D3 K to
12:46
today's episode is also brought To Us by inside tracker inside tracker is a personalized nutrition platform. The analyzes data from your blood and DNA to help you better understand your body and help you reach your health goals.
12:58
I've long been a
12:59
Believer in getting regular blood work done. For the simple reason that many of the factors that influence your immediate and long-term Health can only be discovered from a quality blood test and nowadays with the Advent of modern
13:11
DNA test. You can also get
13:13
information for instance, about how your biological age compares to your chronological age. Which, of course, is a vital measurement. Now,
13:21
one of the major issues with blood tests and DNA tests out. There, is that people get the
13:25
information back that
13:27
lipid marker
13:28
of one type in
13:29
Be high or low or that a hormone and of another type might be high or low,
13:33
but they don't give you any information about what to do with
13:35
that information inside tracker makes that all very easy to navigate. Once you get your results back. You can click on any of those results.
13:43
And inside tracker will immediately show you things that you can do
13:46
for instance, with your nutrition or supplementation or lifestyle
13:49
factors to help you bring those numbers into the ranges that are appropriate for you. So it's immensely powerful, not just in terms of the measurements, but also it
13:57
provides some directives that can help.
13:59
Bring those measurements into the ranges that are best for your immediate and long-term Health.
14:04
If you like to try inside tracker, you can visit inside tracker, dot, coms huberman, to get 20% off any of
14:09
inside. Trackers plans. Just use the code huberman at
14:12
checkout. Let's talk about desire, love and attachment.
14:16
Of course, these are topics that grab tremendous
14:18
interest. So it's worth us defining our terms a little bit
14:21
before going, any further.
14:23
Of course, we can have many different kinds
14:25
of loves. There's romantic love. There's love of family.
14:29
The so called familial love, there's love of pets. We can even love
14:33
objects where we can feel. As if we love objects. We can love certain activities. We can have friends that we love and so on and so forth. The word love is used to Encompass a lot of different types of relationships
14:44
today. We are mainly going to be focused on romantic love
14:47
and the neural mechanisms of romantic
14:50
love. I want to acknowledge here at the outset that most of the studies of romantic love have focused on monogamous
14:56
heterosexual. Love and also,
14:59
Oh, it, when we talk about studies focused
15:01
on desire and attractiveness and attachment, that's also the case and that simply reflects the general bias of the literature
15:08
over the last 50 to 100 years. It does, of course, not rule out that
15:12
similar or different
15:13
mechanisms could be at play in non-monogamous relationships, in homosexual, relationships,
15:19
were in relationships of any kind or variation. It's also Worth US defining. Our terms around desire. It can
15:26
mean lust, it can mean the desire for long.
15:29
From partnership. So we need to Define our terms and throughout, I will do my best to very carefully. Define what I mean by
15:36
desire, what I mean by love and what I mean by attachment,
15:41
the formal study of
15:44
Love and Desire and
15:45
attachment goes back to
15:47
the early 1900s. One of the classic studies of on, this is entitled Love and Desire. It was published in 1912 and really focused on
15:57
two opposing themes.
15:59
Within romance. One is love, which in that paper was really meant to include attachment and dependents or interdependence between individuals. All right,
16:11
and the other end of the spectrum being
16:13
desire or the sexual desire for another and romance was meant to encapsulate. Both those things Love and Desire and for much of the 1980s. It was thought that Love and
16:26
Desire were on sort of opposing answering kind of a push.
16:29
Or, and it was the dynamic push and pull between
16:32
love and a desire that one could Define
16:37
romance. And that actually led to much of
16:39
what's out there in the psychological
16:41
literature. Today. We are going to explore some neurobiological
16:45
studies, some studies of the endocrine system, using the hormone system
16:49
that actually support that General model. And I'll point you toward a, what I think, is a very useful book in thinking about how
16:56
relationships can both form and last,
16:59
A long periods of time and how those relationships can include both
17:04
desire and interdependence. I'll also talk about some studies that have really focused on why relationships succeed and why they
17:13
fail. And how that relates to whether or not there is sufficient amounts of attachment and desire. So today, we're going to talk about the
17:20
science and indeed, you'll also get some tools, those tools should be useful
17:23
to you whether or not you happen to be in a relationship or not whether or not you're seeking your relationship or not. I'd like to
17:29
In
17:29
with an anecdote. And this is not an anecdote about my relationship history. It's a anecdote about my scientific
17:37
history. When I started graduate school, the chairman of the department. I was
17:41
in at the time, said to me, you know, most
17:44
phds last longer than most marriages and indeed, he
17:47
was right. And also most marriages in this country. And in divorce. I
17:52
think it's about 50% with a slight, skew
17:55
toward more ending in divorce than persist until
17:59
Death do them part, but nonetheless, it's
18:02
about half and most marriages end
18:05
before the eight-year period is up. Most phds.
18:09
Take anywhere from four to nine
18:11
years. So, there was a bit of a smearing of averages there. But the point he was trying to make really landed home for me. It did not scare me of out of relationships, nor did it scare me out of a PhD, obviously, what it did illustrate was that there's something about our attachment Machinery. That can
18:29
be very
18:29
Very compelling such that people take on
18:32
tremendous levels of commitment. I have to imagine the most people enter marriages assuming that they're going to stay in those marriages. I don't think most people enter marriages thinking, they're going to get divorced but
18:41
that if 50% of those commitments end in divorce, there must also be mechanisms
18:47
by which our attachments can break. And
18:50
today we're going to talk about both the forming of
18:52
attachments and the breaking of attachments, what can prevent those brakes and attachments and indeed what can lead to really
18:59
Attachments
19:01
there are
19:02
biological mechanisms to desire, love and attachment. That's
19:07
abundantly clear. Now. There's a robust and very large literature in animal models. And what I mean by that, our field studies and laboratory studies in primates of different
19:18
kinds such as macaque monkeys or bonobos people have looked at these sorts of things believe or not in ducks. In laboratory,
19:26
mice in different types of
19:27
birds,
19:28
Etc.
19:29
You look at that literature. You can essentially find biological examples in the animal kingdom for just about any behavior that you can easily
19:37
map to human behavior.
19:39
So, for instance, there's a species of animal called the Prairie vole in one portion of the United States. This Prairie vole species is monogamous. They only mate with one other Prairie, vole only
19:51
raise young with one other Prairie vole for their entire life and in another region of the United States that same species of animal.
19:59
The Prairie vole
20:01
will mate, with many individuals. They're non-monogamous. And the major difference at least as far as we know between the Prairie voles in one location to another location, is the levels of
20:11
a molecule called vasopressin in the brain and
20:13
body vasopressin is present in humans. It
20:16
has numerous biological roles. It's an responsible, for instance, for controlling the amount of urine that you
20:21
excrete, the amount of water that you retain and for sexual desire, as well as mate seeking.
20:29
Levels of vasopressin in Prairie voles are strongly determinant of whether or not
20:34
a prairie vole is going to be a monogamous or non-monogamous.
20:38
Why do I raise this? Well, I raise this because the literature on Prairie
20:42
voles is quite beautiful and has been discussed quite a lot in the popular press. You can look it up with an easy easily just web engine, search. You'll find lots of information about this. Lots of news articles about this
20:54
and lots of interpretations as to how vasopressin might be involved in similar, or different.
20:58
In isms, in humans. Now, I don't have a problem with mapping animal studies to humans. I think there's certainly a place for that. But if you just sort of lean back and look at the giant
21:09
mass of studies in animals, and their mating behavior and their mate selection Behavior, you can essentially find examples of anything, you can find
21:18
examples of polygamy. You can find
21:20
examples of cheating, you know, of infidelity, you can find
21:23
examples of all sorts of different behaviors that in your own mind, you can
21:27
map to human behavior.
21:29
But it's really hard to make the leap
21:31
from animal models to humans in any kind of direct way. And so
21:35
thankfully there's been tremendous work done in the last mainly 20 years or so. Looking at human mate. Selection human desire,
21:43
human love and human attachment.
21:45
So were mainly going to focus on those studies today and where appropriate we will map those findings back to the
21:51
findings and animals to see
21:52
if there are some Universal truths or some Universal principles
21:56
about how the neural circuits and bye.
21:58
Logical mechanisms work but by and large we're going to focus on human studies today. So unless I say, otherwise, the data that I'm referring to today are entirely from human beings. So let's talk about attachment and attachment Styles
22:10
and this will offer you the opportunity to answer some important questions for yourself
22:13
such as what is my meaning, your attachment style in relationship.
22:20
One of the most robust
22:23
findings in the field of psychology. Is this notion of attachment Styles,
22:27
and this was something that
22:28
Discovered through a beautiful set of studies that were done
22:31
by Mary Ainsworth, in the 1980s, in which she developed a laboratory condition called The Strange situation task.
22:39
Now, the strange situation task has been studied over and over again in different
22:43
cultures in different
22:45
locations throughout the world. And in preparing for this episode. I actually spoke to three different
22:50
psychologist. I spoke to a psychoanalyst. I spoke to a cognitive behavioral psychologist and I actually spoke to a psychiatrist, use me not a psychologist. But a psychiatrist with a
22:58
School degree and asked
23:00
is the strange situation task and the various attachment styles
23:03
that emerge from that
23:05
task. Are those still considered valid and indeed. All three of them said, If ever there was a literature in
23:11
Psychology that is absolutely tamped down and has a
23:15
firm basis in both data and real world
23:19
principles and real world examples. It's this notion of attachment Styles. So, what is the strange situation task the strange situation to?
23:28
It involves a parent. Typically a mother in the studies that were done, but a parent or other caregiver
23:35
bringing their child, their actual child
23:37
into a laboratory.
23:39
And there's a room with a stranger and the mother enters the room with the child and there's some toys in the room and typically the mother, and the stranger will talk. Obviously, the stranger is part of the experiment is
23:53
not just some random person off the street and the
23:56
child is allowed to move about the room. They can observe.
23:58
Give the mother interacting with the other person or not. They can play with toys or not. But then at some point, the mother leaves.
24:06
And then at some point later, designated by the experimenter, the mother comes back
24:11
and what is measured in these studies, is both how the child the
24:17
toddler reacts to the mother leaving
24:21
and how the child reacts to the mother
24:23
returning, at the end of the
24:24
experiment. And often times. This will have two or three.
24:28
Different phases where the mother will bring the child in then leave then come back in and leave their also studies in which the behavior of the child with the stranger is
24:40
also
24:41
examined. So there are a lot of variations of this, but the basic findings are that toddlers children fall into four different
24:51
categories of attachment style.
24:54
And that these attachment Styles can predict many features of a
24:58
Adolescent teen young adult, and even adult attachment Styles not in strange situations of the sort that I just described. But in romantic
25:07
attachments,
25:10
I should mention also that attachment style is plastic, meaning it can change
25:15
across the lifespan. So as I described the results, I describe the different attachment styles that are out there
25:22
and if any of those resonate with you or bring to mine certain people in your life, please do not assume that those attachment styles are
25:31
rigid and fixed for the entire life span. There are also terrific data that indicate
25:36
that through specific.
25:38
Processes both psychological and some biological adjustments that people can change their attachment style and that indeed people
25:46
who have different attachment Styles can change the attachment styles of others, but just to make very clear what the results of the study were. I want to review what the four different attachment styles are and typically people fall into one group or another? But not several. So the 4 patterns of
26:02
attachment that were revealed by these studies.
26:04
Again, were revealed by examining the behavior of the child and
26:08
Response to the mother, leaving the room and the mother returning, and the child's response to the stranger that is in the room with them.
26:16
The first style is the so-called
26:19
secure attachment
26:20
style in the nomenclature of this kind of study. These are the
26:24
so-called be babies as, in the letter B, Bulldog be not for Bulldogs, but just to designate this category.
26:31
The secure attachment style is one in which the child will engage with the stranger with the experimenter.
26:39
While the parent is present in the room, but that when the parent typically it's a mother but when the parent or other caregiver leaves the child does get
26:48
visibly upset, they might win. They might cry. They might even tantrum a bit.
26:53
However, when the caregiver meaning the mother or father other caregiver Returns the child
27:00
visibly expresses happiness that the caregiver has returned. Okay. So that's the Hallmark of the secure attachment style.
27:07
And again, this is all.
27:08
Pre verbal, this is happening
27:10
long before the child can express, how they feel with words.
27:13
And the interpretation of this is that the secure child feels confident that the caregiver is available and will be responsive to their needs
27:22
and their Communications. So that when the child whines and, whereas, you know,
27:27
distressed the parent doesn't come right back into the room, but at some point, they do, and they seem to have a sense of
27:32
trust that if the parent or caregiver leaves that, the parent will come back and that they're happy that they do.
27:39
These children are also very good at
27:42
exploring novel environments after the parent is gone. And while the parent is there, and
27:47
almost always when the parent is there. They will. Explore More broadly, literally in space. They'll venture out further
27:54
than they will. When the parent is gone.
27:56
They also tend to engage with the caregiver in a way that's not immediately and completely trusting, but that over time, seems to evolve from one in which they're kind of suspicious of this person to one in which they are at least somewhat.
28:09
Trusting. Okay. So those are the general Contours of the secure attachment style and fortunately. Nowadays. There are physiological, studies measuring things like heart
28:17
rate and breathing and other measures that correlate with
28:21
the subjective assessment of what these children are feeling.
28:25
Okay. So first category, secure attachment, the second category is a so-called anxious
28:29
avoidant or insecurely attached, which are the category a baby's,
28:35
the chop the children with anxious, avoid an insecure attachment patterns.
28:39
I generally tend to avoid or
28:41
ignore the caregiver. All right, meaning the
28:44
parent and show very little emotion when the parent leaves
28:50
or returns. So this is the reason they call them avoidant or anxious avoidant and kind of insecure. There isn't this happiness or joy that the parent is back. They don't seem to express that
29:01
they do not exhibit distress on separation and they generally tend to have some tendency to approach.
29:09
The, the caregiver when they return. But it, but there doesn't seem to be
29:12
a general expression of joy. And again, physiological, measures support that as well. Things like, changes in heart rate tend to be less dramatic in the anxious avoidant, insecure attachment style than in the secure attachment style. Okay. So that's the second one.
29:28
The third category is the so-called anxious. Ambivalent / resistant
29:33
insecure category. I didn't name these categories so you have to blame others and
29:39
In this one instance for everything else, blame me. But in this instance, you have to blame the psychologist that name this
29:44
category. The anxious ambivalent / resistant insecure category also called The Sea
29:49
Babies for the letter c. Just as a
29:52
categorization, the anxious ambivalent resistant insecure
29:57
toddlers, really
29:58
show distress, even
30:00
before separation from their mother or other caregiver.
30:03
And they tend to be very clingy and difficult to
30:05
comfort when the caregiver returns.
30:09
So their
30:09
distress even before the mother leaves the room and they tend to be very clingy and really hard to
30:14
calm down when the mother returns.
30:18
They tend to show either, what seems to be resentment in response to the parents absence. We don't really know what they're feeling or some sort of helpless passivity. And there's actually some
30:27
categorizations that the psychologists have come up with, with C1 subtypes and see, two subtypes. We don't have to get bogged down in that
30:33
but just know that there isn't one absolute
30:35
measure that says, oh well, this person.
30:39
Person is anxious ambivalent resistant insecure.
30:41
They could be somewhat passive,
30:43
or they could be seem somewhat angry at the caregiver. But the basic idea is
30:48
that before, and
30:50
after the separation, they are clingy and difficult to comfort. They just can't seem to calm themselves down and physiological measures of heart rate and hormone measurements. Such as cortisol also, support that statement
31:02
and the third category of attachment style is the so-called disorganized or disoriented
31:08
or D.
31:09
For the letter D. Baby's this is a categorization that was added later to this strange situation task. That is a real Hallmark of Developmental Psychology
31:18
studies. It was developed by Mary. Ainsworth graduate student.
31:23
Mary main who actually had the great Fortune of taking a course from and learning from when I was a graduate student at Berkeley many years ago
31:30
and this fourth categorization was controversial for a while, but now is generally
31:34
accepted the
31:36
key feature of the disorganized.
31:39
Rented category is that the toddler's tend to be tense? And they tend to Encompass a lot of kind
31:46
of odd. Physical postures. They tend to
31:48
hunch their shoulders.
31:49
They'll put their hands behind their
31:50
neck. They'll cock their head to the side. For those of you listening. I'm doing this on the video
31:56
version. It's not where you don't have to go see that. But it for those of you that are watching this on video. They tend to kind of
32:02
constrain their, their body size a bit and going to AUD postures that they normally wouldn't do anywhere else.
32:10
So this is why it's called the disorganized or
32:12
disoriented category. It seems like these children just don't really know how to react to a separation and they just start to
32:20
manifest behaviors and emotional tones.
32:23
That aren't observed in other situations.
32:25
Okay. So we've got our four categories. I'll try and use the shortest possible names for each category. We've got category 1 which is
32:31
securely attached. We've got category
32:33
to, which is insecurely attached. Also sometimes
32:36
called anxious avoidant,
32:38
then we've got
32:38
Cory three, which is the resistant insecure
32:42
category, which is anxious, ambivalent. And then
32:44
there's this fourth category. The disorganized
32:46
disoriented category where the so-called D babies. Now,
32:52
what's interesting about this from the
32:53
perspective of Desire, love and attachment. Is that the
32:58
categorizations of children into one
33:01
of these four different categories as toddlers
33:05
is strongly predictive of their attachment style in romantic.
33:09
Partnerships later in
33:10
life, which is to me, both amazing and
33:14
surprising and not surprising all at the same
33:16
time, amazing.
33:17
Because it means that first of all, we are relatively hard wired for attachment. I think that that's incredible and beautiful that we have designated neurons nerve cells and
33:29
hormonal systems that are there to ensure that we have some sort of
33:33
response to a caregiver being there or not being there or returning or
33:38
Or leaving
33:40
but also that the same neural. Circuitry. He's the same. Hormonal responses are, at least in some way repurposed for entirely different types of
33:49
attachments later in life.
33:51
So when we hear the psychologists talk about how, you know, we formed a
33:56
template early in life, based on experiences that were even pre verbal before we had language and those
34:02
templates are superimposed on our relationships or
34:05
we should say our later relationships are superimposed on those templates.
34:09
There really is a basis for that. We now have neuroimaging studies to support. For instance, the work
34:14
of Alain Shore from UCLA showing that
34:17
when a mother and child interact,
34:20
either through very soothing interactions, like, bottle feeding or breastfeeding, or
34:25
singing to one's baby, or putting them to sleep that the brain of the child, and the brain of the mother are entering a coordinated state of relaxation, and it's not One
34:35
Direction mother to child. The child is also calming the
34:38
Mother typically these studies were done with mothers again sometimes with fathers but typically with mothers
34:45
and in addition to that when the mother or other caregiver acts very excited and raises their voice or puts a lilt in their voice or widens their eyes that the child will do
34:55
the same. And again, there's a bi-directional interaction in that case of excitement and there's the release of neurochemicals like dopamine into the bloodstream. Whereas in the relaxation scenario and the soothing
35:07
scenario. There's we know the
35:09
The release of things like serotonin and oxytocin.
35:11
So the neural systems for attachment and the neural
35:16
systems, for what we call autonomic arousal, for being alert and calm
35:21
don't act in a vacuum. They are Tethered to other people in our environment. And of course, we know this, right? We sometimes hear the statement. No one can make you feel anything. I've always had a little
35:31
bit of a problem with that statement. I don't think I'm contradicting anyone in particular, but you hear that a lot. No one can make you feel.
35:38
Anything indeed, they can write a physical injury can make you feel something.
35:43
If somebody says something that you
35:44
very much like it can make you feel something. And if somebody says something that you very much dislike,
35:49
it will make you feel something. So, the idea that no one can make us feel anything isn't actually true. Our nervous system is
35:55
Tethered to the nervous systems of others. And that is
35:58
true from the
35:59
very earliest stages of our lives. And in this
36:02
case, we're talking about how our templates for attachment in romantic relationships, how we find them how we maintain.
36:08
Them and indeed how we break them and reform. Them is based on a template that was established through an entirely different set of priorities, which was how we feel safe and
36:18
secure in novel environments. Depending on whether or not our primary caregiver is there or not
36:23
neuroimaging supports that when I say neuroimaging, I mean brain scan support that measures of hormones in
36:28
the body and brain support that measures of neurochemical support. That there's
36:32
simply no way around this truth that we have a set of road maps in our mind that are reused for
36:38
An entirely different purposes later in
36:40
life that is vitally important to understand because
36:45
if one is successful in
36:46
forming romantic attachments maintaining them Etc
36:51
or not does in fact reflect the earlier
36:55
templates that you've
36:56
created but as I've mentioned before, the good news is that these templates can shift over time and one of the more powerful ways
37:03
to shift those templates over time is
37:05
purely by the knowledge that they exist. And the
37:08
In that those templates are malleable. They can change through the process of
37:14
neuroplasticity. Again, neuroplasticity is just a rewiring of nerve connections. That is very much present in childhood, but also very much present in
37:23
adulthood. So if you're somebody who you think
37:26
falls in the category, 1 2 3 or 4 or, you know, somebody or involved with somebody who falls in the category, 1 2, 3 and 4.
37:34
The mere knowledge of that can be very
37:36
useful but you might ask. What do I do with that knowledge? Well, fortunately both psychologists and biologists have started to leverage that knowledge toward establishing better, more secure Bonds in adult romantic relationships and there's a book that has really tapped into this. I think it's the first book that is really address this head-on and that book comes from to Columbia professors. And the title of the book is
38:00
attached.
38:02
The new science of adult attachment.
38:04
How it can help you find and keep love that. The authors of this book are a mere Levine and Rachel Heller again. Both of them are skilled academics and researchers who have really taken the literature that I described on the
38:17
strange situation to ask and mapped it to adult attachment Styles.
38:21
And also they've mapped out ways that they've observed in their clinical practice and that
38:26
is laboratory supported for,
38:29
for instance, people that have an anxious
38:31
ambivalent or what we would call an insecure attachment.
38:34
Dial or four people that fall into the disorganized or disoriented attachment style, how they can modify that attachment style
38:42
in or out
38:43
of relationships in order to establish. What I think everybody wants which is secure attachment. Why does everybody
38:49
want that? Well, secure attachment allows people to be both in
38:53
relationship or if they choose to be on their own
38:56
or to be in relationship, but
38:57
physically separated from somebody else or even emotionally, separated from somebody
39:01
else. And maintain what we call a stable autonomic.
39:04
Make equilibrium the ability
39:06
to remain calm clear-headed. You might not like what's happening,
39:10
but you're able to navigate that with some sense of clarity and not excessive discomfort. So, is there a
39:15
goal in all of this stuff? About love desire and attachment indeed. There is the the secure attachment style is the one that
39:24
leads to the most stable and predictable long-term relationships put differently.
39:30
Babies, toddlers adolescents, teens and young adults that have a secure. Attachment style are more likely to find in form long-term relationships than are people in the other categories,
39:40
but people in other categories can learn and eventually migrate into the
39:45
secure attachment style.
39:47
And I think that book attached. I have no affiliation to the
39:50
author's or the book itself. I should just mention that attach the new science of adult attachment and how it can help you find and
39:56
keep low. Really? It sounds very
39:59
Psychology ask but it is really grounded in the research psychology literature. And there's also some interesting biology there.
40:06
Another point to make about attachment Styles, is that it
40:11
is possible
40:12
and some of you may be familiar with circumstances, where by people who are securely attached
40:18
either, because they grew up in an environment where secure attachment was
40:20
cultivated or because they developed that on their own can
40:24
also migrate out of the securely attached.
40:28
Category into insecurely attached or into avoidant types of attachment Styles as teens or as young adults, or, as adults of any age, or any stage of Life,
40:40
by virtue of being with somebody who has a different
40:43
perhaps less adaptive attachment style.
40:46
Right? What this means is that if
40:48
you have or you develop a secure attachment style,
40:52
it's vitally important to
40:54
protect that attachment style because it is possible to become anxiously.
40:58
Even if you grew up in a stable attachment framework and again this can happen at any stage. So if you're interested in attachment Styles, and how they influence adult romantic attachments, and certainly, if you
41:09
are a parent, I would encourage you to check out the
41:13
book attached again, it's quite good and I think that it offers a number of actionable
41:18
tools to both form
41:20
and hold on to secure attachment Styles. So I mentioned that the neural circuits for child parent or child care giver attachment.
41:28
Are repurposed for romantic attachment later in life. But what are these neural circuits? What do they do? I mean it's so attractive if you will to think about a brain area that controls love or a brain area that controls desire or a brain area that controls attaching. But it simply doesn't work that way
41:47
as I've talked about before on this podcast and I will say again and again because it will persist to be true long after I'm gone is that no one brain
41:55
area can give rise to any thing is
41:58
Complexes desire. Lover attachment
42:01
instead, there are multiple brain
42:02
areas that through their coordinated
42:04
action. Create a sort of a
42:06
song that we call Desire, or a song that we call love or a song that we call attachment not a literal song. Although there are songs about desire love and attachment. Of course many songs some good, some not so good.
42:19
But rather different brain areas being active in different sequences and with different intensities can make us feel as
42:26
if we are in the
42:28
The mode that we call Desire in the mode of Love or in the mode of attachment, but beneath
42:33
all of that is this element
42:36
of autonomic arousal
42:37
and I want to focus on autonomic arousal just for a bit longer because it really is one of the
42:43
Three core Elements by which we form and maintain loving attachments and by which we break loving attachments
42:51
the autonomic nervous system
42:53
as the name suggests is automatic. In fact, that's what.
42:58
Comic means now, it's actually the case that we can control our autonomic nervous system to some degree or
43:03
another, but the autonomic nervous system, controls things like digestion are breathing
43:09
whether or not, we're conscious of that breathing or not. It controls things, like how alert we are, or how sleepy we are,
43:15
and the autonomic nervous system. As I just briefly described earlier, is really something that we come into the world
43:22
with it's hardwired. All the elements are there but
43:25
through interactions with our parents.
43:28
Aunt either soothing interactions or fun playful interactions or dare, I say scary interactions. Are autonomic nervous system gets tuned up. Meaning we each develop a tendency to either be more alert and anxious or more calm or kind of a
43:45
balance of alert and calm. Now, of course, this changes across each day and depending on tired, we are late in the day. If we've been awake for a while we tend to get sleepy early in the day. We're very rested. We tend to wake up and feel very alert. So the
43:56
way to think about the autonomic nervous,
43:58
Is it kind of a seesaw?
43:59
We go back and forth
44:00
between being very alert. We can be alert and calm or we can be very, very alert. We can be in a state of panic, we can be fast asleep. So we can be
44:08
extremely calm, or we can just
44:09
be kind of sleepy semi calm and but still also alert,
44:14
so think about it like a
44:15
seesaw and that seesaw has a
44:18
hinge and that hinge defines how tight or
44:22
loose. That seesaw is how readily it can tilt back and forth.
44:26
Our autonomic tone.
44:28
Is how
44:28
tight that hinges and there are biological mechanisms to explain this. But here I just want to stay with the analogy of the Seesaw for now.
44:36
The interactions between child
44:39
and caregiver early in
44:40
life, take the child and the caregiver from one end of the Seesaw to the
44:46
other, from being very Alert, in a state of play, for instance,
44:50
to being nursed and being very soothing until we go to
44:53
sleep. And of course, we each have a seesaw, the parent on the child has a seesaw and they're interacting. What do I mean by that?
44:59
Well, there are
45:00
beautiful studies, and beautiful
45:02
and not in the sense of a focused on a pleasant
45:04
topic. But
45:05
Because they were done so beautifully. Well,
45:08
that looked at, for instance, the response of mothers and their physiologies and the response of children and their physiologies during the bombing of cities during World War
45:19
Two. So an unpleasant
45:20
situation, but what was revealed during the course of these studies was that if the mothers were very stressed during an
45:29
onslaught of bombing of the city, the
45:31
children's physiologies tended to be stressed also and
45:36
Persisted in being stressed, long after that stressful
45:38
episode was done. They actually followed that these children. Well out for many decades afterwards.
45:47
Conversely, if the
45:49
parent and in this case, again, it was Mother's that that were explored in these studies
45:53
had turned this whole business of going into the bomb shelters into somewhat of a game. All right. Taking it seriously, but essentially telling the children, okay, it's time to go but not expressing much stress or
46:04
distress.
46:05
Children also didn't develop much stress, or distress, or trauma. Now, there were exceptions
46:10
to this, of course, but in general, that was the rule that the autonomic nervous systems of children. Tend to
46:15
mimic the autonomic nervous systems of the primary caregiver and the mechanisms by which this occurs has been
46:21
explored. And again, I just referred to the beautiful work of Alain Shore, at University of California, Los Angeles. I mean, again, his name is is sure, spelled
46:30
SCH o re I'm looking down, briefly at the floor here because I'll just reach for the
46:35
Book, he has a wonderful book called right brain, Psychotherapy. It's a
46:39
little bit technical. But if you're
46:41
interested in some of the studies,
46:43
this book, right? Being
46:44
Psychotherapy details,
46:45
how everything from nursing of children to play time, Behavior to strange situation type task behavior that we talked about
46:53
before which of course, occurs when children get dropped off at daycare or nursery school, or with babysitters
46:58
Etc. And indeed all
47:01
types of caregiver, child
47:03
interactions tune-up that autonomic
47:05
A system.
47:06
So that the child ends up with an autonomic nervous system that either tends to lean more towards alert and anxious, or can be very
47:15
alert, but calm, or can be very calm and hard to budge.
47:18
Again. It's the tightness of that hinge that
47:21
really underlies. These attachment styles that we were talking about earlier
47:25
and not on this episode of
47:26
the human lamp podcast, but on many other previous episodes such as the master stress episode or some of the optimize Health episodes. You can find these if you want.
47:35
Huberman lab.com.
47:36
A lot of the tools and techniques that are recommended. There have to do with readjusting
47:42
the autonomic nervous system in deliberate ways as an adult.
47:45
Again. I won't go into the the specific tools, but for instance, the the physiological sigh this tool
47:53
that I've talked about extensively of to inhales
47:56
through the nose as deeply as you can, on the first one sneaking in a little bit more are on the second one and then a long exhale through the mouth is a way of adjusting
48:04
that autonomic. See saw it.
48:05
Tends to make us more calm. It activates, what we call the parasympathetic arm
48:10
of the autonomic nervous
48:11
system, which is just fancy nerd. Speak for it's a quick way to calm yourself down.
48:15
Right things, like ice baths or cold, showers or cold immersion or hyperventilate. Deliberate, hyperventilation by contrast or ways in which, we can deliberately increase the level of our so-called sympathetic
48:30
arm of our autonomic nervous system. Make ourselves more alert.
48:32
Why would you want to do that? Well, you can do
48:34
that to be more alert.
48:35
To be more awake
48:36
if you like, or as a form of self induced stress, inoculation to be able
48:40
to tolerate higher levels of adrenaline, by making it a practice that you self
48:45
direct. The reason those tools are out. There is because many of us for whatever reason we don't have to blame anyone but because of our childhood templates because of things that happened in didn't
48:58
happen. In terms of our interactions with caregivers have autonomic
49:01
nervous systems that are tilted to one side or the other more than we would like or
49:06
In which the hinge
49:07
that I'm talking about in this analogy is too loose
49:11
or that is too tight. And we're sort of stuck in a mode of anxiousness or stuck in a mode
49:15
of lack of energy. That's what those tools are really
49:18
about. But at a deeper level, the autonomic nervous system is really the system that governs how we will react in
49:28
response to a romantic partner, being present or leaving. And I don't necessarily mean leaving the relationship.
49:35
Entirely, although it could mean
49:37
that right. We know people, I'm sure, you know, people that upon the end of a relationship that they wanted very much are absolutely crushed. And actually, in researching this episode there. I discovered, there's an extensive literature, finding that the feelings that one has. After a breakup are very much like a clinical
49:55
depression in many cases,
49:57
but there are individuals that can look at a breakup as a transient event that they don't interpret as going to mean so much.
50:05
For all aspects of their life for reshaping their view of themselves.
50:09
Why? Well, we have different levels of autonomic function and depending on where our seesaw is if you will, some of us become extremely distraught and can't recalibrate ourselves. Can't
50:22
adjust ourselves down from, you know, stress to calm or can't take ourselves from exhausted to more alert if we need to do that on our own. And so that's why tools to for doing that exists. But
50:33
attachment itself.
50:35
Is about where our autonomic nervous system resides. So if I were to offer a set of
50:40
Tools around, these topics of desired love and attachment, I
50:44
would say, first of all, you might want to
50:46
think about whether or not you fall into the secure insecure or other attachment Styles,
50:50
second. I think it is vitally important for all of us, but certainly for people that are in relationships or seeking relationships to be able to, at least have some recognition of where our autonomic nervous system
51:05
tends.
51:05
To reside both in terms of when we are with
51:08
somebody. And when they leave, when we are apart for long periods of time, can we calm ourselves? Can we self-soothe or are we very
51:16
much dependent on the presence of another in order to feel soothed?
51:19
Now, I absolutely want to emphasize that there is nothing wrong. In fact, there's everything right with feeling great
51:26
in the presence of somebody else. That is
51:28
actually a Hallmark of
51:29
strong and quality attachments. These
51:32
days, we hear the term
51:33
codependent a lot. This was a
51:35
I believe the term was first coined by Pia Melody and it actually does occupy an important role
51:41
in the
51:42
world of trauma. Trauma healing. So called trauma bonding
51:46
topics of another episode. I actually did an episode on fear and Trauma and we will do one all about trauma bonding with an expert at some point in the
51:52
future, but codependence and codependency. The term can sometimes be misinterpreted as any dependence on another isn't good. Interdependence healthy.
52:05
This, of course, is good. It is the Hallmark of healthy child,
52:08
parent relations, sibling, relations and romantic relations, but a key element of healthy interdependence is that? Yes, our autonomic nervous
52:17
system is adjusted by the presence of another, but
52:20
that also that we can adjust
52:22
our own autonomic nervous system even in the absence of that person
52:26
that if the person goes away temporarily or permanently that we can still
52:31
regulate our own autonomic nervous system both
52:33
from states of stress, to
52:35
States of
52:35
Calm, both from states of exhaustion to states of more alertness. And of course, we all need sleep to go from exhaustion to alertness. But what I'm
52:44
referring to here is the ability to regulate when distraught or
52:48
regulate when fatigued or feeling
52:52
depressed and that is an is all about
52:55
the autonomic nervous system. So as we talk about attachment Styles, we talk about
52:59
infant and toddler and adult attachment Styles. What we are really talking about is a complex set of neural circuitry.
53:06
And one of those neural circuitry is which is absolutely crucial. Is this autonomic nervous system.
53:11
So if the autonomic nervous system is one key component
53:14
of Desire, love and attachment. What are the other two? And what I'm going to tell you next is largely. The pioneering work of Helen Fisher,
53:24
who is really an anthropologist who's become a bit of a neuroscientist in this? Collaborated with neuroscientists to establish brain areas and neural circuits that are associated with different aspects of
53:35
moment, Love and
53:36
Desire. I think the first really high quality study of neural circuits associated with these themes,
53:42
was a paper published in 2005. In a very fine. Anatomical Journal, perhaps, the best
53:48
neuroanatomical Journal which is the Journal of comparative neurology.
53:53
The Journal of comparative neurology has been around for more than 100 years and is
53:57
considered the archival location for placing really high quality Anatomy. They have tremendously high standards and the study.
54:05
That I'm referring to is entitled romantic love and fmri,
54:09
meaning functional magnetic resonance. Imaging
54:11
study of a neural mechanism for
54:13
mate? Choice. And dr. Fisher is a author on this paper as
54:19
is Arthur Aron and Lucy Brown. So all very fine researchers and this study as well as several other studies using
54:28
magnetic resonance. Imaging, things like EG neuron, Tomica
54:32
tracing Etc have identified a large number of brain areas.
54:35
That are associated with different aspects of desired, love and attachment. And I'll just throw out a few names of those brain areas and what they control. And then I'll tell you how those anchor
54:43
to the other two categories of neural circuits essential for desired love and attachment. So
54:49
not surprisingly, the dopamine system
54:52
in the brain, is associated with desire, love and attachment and mainly with desire although to some extent love
54:58
dopamine is a neurotransmitter will sometimes associated with reward, but as some of you have heard me say before it is made.
55:05
The a molecule of motivation, craving and
55:08
pursuit and
55:09
that motivation, craving a Pursuit that
55:12
relates to dopamine is not unique to attachment, or love or sex or mating etcetera. It is a
55:17
universal generic
55:19
currency in the brain for pursuing something food. When you're hungry, a mate when you want, 12, May 20 want to
55:27
warmth when you're cold etcetera, Etc. Okay, so it's not for one specific purpose, but the brain areas associated with
55:33
dopamine involve.
55:36
For instance, the ventral tegmental area, the substantia nigra areas of that
55:40
sort. The basal ganglia. You don't need to know these names, just understand that. These are networks of neurons that tend to put the person you into a state
55:49
of forward action and pursuit and craving and motivation. They are not about being quiet and relaxed etcetera.
55:57
The neural circuits for quiescence and relaxation are most associated with love and
56:04
attachment not surprisingly.
56:05
And there the neuro chemical serotonin and to some extent, oxytocin
56:09
are the predominant neurochemicals involved and those are released from brain areas such as the raphe nucleus in the back of the brain. You may have heard that the majority of Serotonin, your body is made in your gut. And indeed that's
56:21
true, but I hate to break it to you. The serotonin, your gut is not responsible for your feelings
56:27
of love and attachment. At least not to a high degree. That's mainly going to be the reflection of neurons in your brain that make
56:34
serotonin and there.
56:35
Other areas of the brain that makes serotonin
56:37
as well, and oxytocin
56:38
as well, but they tend to be associated with the kind of warmth and calm and the
56:44
soothing that we feel in the presence of another.
56:46
And again, these are not strictly divided circuits.
56:50
We can have dopamine and serotonin present in our brain and body at the same time to equal or very, or different
56:55
degrees. And we were turning a little bit to what happens when levels of
56:59
dopamine are very high and low levels of Serotonin or low and vice versa and so on, including in States of
57:05
Neuro chemically modified States as it were in when we talk about things like MDMA so-called
57:12
Ecstasy. But, in the meantime, I want to just discuss the to neural circuits that use dopamine that
57:20
you serotonin and oxytocin and that collaborate with the autonomic nervous system to drive, what we call Desire love and attachment
57:29
and the three circuits
57:32
are autonomic nervous system. We talked about that one.
57:35
Then there's the nervous system components or the neural circuits for empathy for being able to see and respond to
57:43
and indeed match the emotional tone or the autonomic tone of another and then there's the third category and this might surprise some of you. It certainly
57:52
surprised me but the data point to the fact that the third neural circuit that's very important for
57:58
establishing bonds is one associated with positive delusions. So
58:02
given that the neural circuits for empathy
58:04
are absolutely crucial.
58:05
All for falling in love and maintaining stable attachments. I'd like to talk about those neural circuits and what they are
58:13
now often when we hear empathy, we think oh, empathy is really about listening to,
58:18
and really understanding what somebody else is feeling, maybe even feeling what they're feeling
58:23
and indeed that's the case. But what do we
58:26
mean by that? Right? What is it to feel? What another feels. Well, what
58:30
it means is that
58:31
there seesaw is driving your seesaw.
58:36
Or your seesaw is somehow driving their seesaw that there's a match in terms of the tilt of those
58:42
seesaws. Now, it doesn't have to be an exact match. Right? If someone that you really care about is very, very
58:46
stressed. You could also become very stressed. That's a form of empathic matching. And there are indeed neural circuits for that. I'll describe what those neural circuits are in a
58:56
moment. But sometimes the best role for us to take is actually one in which
59:01
we are calm when the person that we care about or that we were romantically involved with
59:05
Is very, very anxious. And in a few minutes,
59:08
I'll talk about how matching of emotional tone can be good or bad
59:13
for the stability of a relationship and complementarity of autonomic matching can be good or bad.
59:21
In other words. Sometimes it's beneficial for a
59:24
relationship to go into the same state as the other.
59:27
And sometimes it's more beneficial for us to not go
59:30
into the same state as the other.
59:33
But the important feature here.
59:35
Here is that when we talk about
59:37
emotional matching or empathy or going into the same state or not going into the same state. What we really talking about is
59:44
whether or not the autonomic seesaw of one individual is
59:48
driving, the autonomic seesaw of the other individual.
59:52
And this is a vital principle for how we fall in
59:56
love and form attachments and it's actually part of the desire and mating process itself.
1:00:02
I would go so far as to say that one of the prerequisites to the
1:00:08
propagation and expansion of our
1:00:10
species is this notion of
1:00:13
autonomic regulation and to some extent matching of autonomic nervous systems. Let me explain what I mean. Last, I checked
1:00:22
the only way that new humans can be created is by way of sperm
1:00:25
meeting egg, either in body, or in dish,
1:00:29
but sperm meets egg, and then
1:00:32
Typically nine months later. We have a human, baby.
1:00:36
The process of bringing sperm to egg, right? Mating behavior sex behavior in humans is one of
1:00:44
autonomic regulation. And
1:00:48
what I mean by that is the process of finding
1:00:53
a mate. And in this case, I mean, actually someone to mate with
1:00:56
typically while scenarios very typically is one of elevated autonomic arousal. Meaning increased activation of the so-called
1:01:05
sympathetic.
1:01:06
This system. This is related to
1:01:07
dopamine release and it's
1:01:09
related to
1:01:10
epinephrine release. There has to be a pursuit or at least. There has to be a mobilization to arrive in the same
1:01:17
location where by one can mate, right? That, that almost always is the case.
1:01:22
However, the sexual arousal itself is in both males, and females is actually driven primarily by the
1:01:36
Pathetic arm of the autonomic nervous
1:01:37
system. So while Pursuit is
1:01:41
One of alertness and
1:01:42
sympathetic drive. As we say again, sympathy is not really what's at play here that the word
1:01:48
simply means together and the
1:01:50
activation of the autonomic nervous system toward more alert state is
1:01:54
because of a sympathetic nervous system in the co activation together of many
1:01:58
neurons in the brain and spinal cord,
1:02:00
but then the actual physiological
1:02:03
arousal state that we call sexual arousal is predominantly parasympathetic. Lee driven. Okay to be quite direct
1:02:10
about this.
1:02:11
If the sympathetic nervous system activation is too high, the sexual arousal, response cannot
1:02:16
happen in either males or in females. It's inhibited.
1:02:20
However, the orgasm and ejaculation response, which if you think about it is required for sperm to meet egg.
1:02:31
Is sympathetic driven. And then after
1:02:35
orgasm, and ejaculation the parasympathetic nervous system kicks back in, and there's a calming and relaxation. So, the
1:02:43
Arc of mating involves sympathetic arousal. Okay, not sympathy, but alertness, and arousal for Pursuit, then a tilt of the Seesaw, at least, to some degree for arousal of the sort that we typically hear of of sexual arousal then.
1:03:00
At the point of orgasm and ejaculation is back to a sympathetic response and why how can I say that? How do I know that the sympathetic nervous system meaning neurons within the sympathetic arm of the autonomic nervous system, are what?
1:03:14
Drive ejaculation and
1:03:15
orgasm and then afterward, there's a return to increased
1:03:21
parasympathetic activation and
1:03:24
we don't know for sure why that happens. But it's thought that in species that pair bond.
1:03:30
Humans, generally pair, but not always there. The return to more parasympathetic activation
1:03:37
after orgasm. And ejaculation, is thought to increase the exchange of
1:03:40
pheromonal orders. Odors scuse me and to increase pillow talk, and pair bonding of different kinds. Okay. So that's the, the, the Seesaw going back and forth is actually built into the
1:03:53
process by which our entire species propagates.
1:03:57
So, in some ways, every human is
1:04:00
Required to navigate that process if they want their offspring to persist.
1:04:05
And of course nowadays, there are Technologies like in vitro fertilization and ginger, you intrauterine insemination. There are a variety of
1:04:13
ways that technology is allow people to circumvent the actual physical mating process in the way that I described but
1:04:19
by and large, that's the
1:04:20
way it's done and certainly that's the way it was done. Historically for, if not tens of thousands or hundreds of thousands of years that process is
1:04:28
also what
1:04:30
opens in
1:04:31
all mammalian species that mate. Okay. So I'm
1:04:34
overlooking an entire literature
1:04:36
of animal studies. That classic studies of this were done by two individuals. I'll just briefly mention them. In case you want to look at the literature. There's a guy at the Rockefeller University named. Donald 5pf
1:04:49
AFF, who has done beautiful studies
1:04:52
identifying, the neural circuitry. What's called the lordosis response.
1:04:56
Unlike in humans, the mating behavior of animals is rather.
1:05:00
In terms of the positions that
1:05:01
they occupy and the lordosis responses, a kind of a you shaping, or a bending up of the hindquarters of typically of rodents, but of other
1:05:09
animals as well. The male the male
1:05:11
mounting is almost always from behind except in some species of primates and that lordosis response is only going to occur during particular phases of
1:05:20
the estrous cycle. The Ester cycle is sort of the analog to the menstrual cycle, but it's not 28 days. It's four days or some other duration in a
1:05:30
other animals depending on the animal.
1:05:32
The lordosis response is strongly regulated by odors by contact and is estrogen and testosterone controlled. And then the male portion of of the mating sequence in animals, the mounting and thrusting, and ejaculation, as they're called, or mounting thrusting, intromission and
1:05:49
ejaculation. Those are the four scientific categories that have been described that's presence in rodents. And also in dogs where it was primarily studied by Frank Beach who was at
1:06:00
you
1:06:00
See, California Berkeley for a long time and the entire literature around the neural circuitry for sexual meaning behavior in animals.
1:06:08
Largely stemmed from the work of Donald Pfaff and Frank Beach and their scientific Offspring not their actual Offspring. You can look at that literature. If you like
1:06:19
there have been human neuroimaging studies of the process
1:06:23
that I described a few minutes ago Believe It or Not of
1:06:27
people in brain scanners not necessarily mating with
1:06:30
A other people, but going through that Arc of arousal,
1:06:36
sympathetic activation during orgasm, or ejaculation. And then the post ejaculatory or orgasmic phase in both men and women and the
1:06:44
Brain areas associated with those have all been mapped out. Now. The spinal cord areas that control things like
1:06:53
erection, vaginal lubrication ejaculation and orgasm. Those have also been mapped
1:06:58
out and this is all been explored from the
1:07:00
A vote of both basic science just to get an understanding of how our species has sexual interactions and reproduces.
1:07:07
But also from the perspective of, for instance, trying to repair, sexual function, after spinal
1:07:13
cord injury, which is, you know, a prominent concern for a lot of people depending on where they have their injury, but in, in the number of people that have spinal cord injuries,
1:07:22
so this is both vital
1:07:24
biological and clinical data
1:07:28
the neural circuits.
1:07:30
For everything that I just described reside in the autonomic nervous system and are coordinated with the neural circuits that are associated with empathy. The neural circuits for empathy again. There are many, but mainly to structures that you should know about the prefrontal cortex, which is how we perceive things
1:07:46
outside of us and make decisions on the basis of those perceptions. How we organize those decisions
1:07:51
and an area of the brain called, the insula i.n.s. Ula, the insula is a really interesting brain area that allows us to interact.
1:08:00
Aricept to pay attention to what's going on inside our body and to split some of our attention to external cept and the mating dance. Whether or not it's
1:08:10
the dinner and date portion of the mating dance, or the actual
1:08:13
physical Dance part of the main dance or actual
1:08:15
mating and, and sexual
1:08:17
behavior kissing. Or otherwise, that is a coordinated activity of two
1:08:24
bodies. Typically its do I realized sometimes it's more. Sometimes it's only
1:08:28
one, but typically, it's two bodies.
1:08:30
And at least in the framework we're using here that coordinated dance is one in which the autonomic nervous system of one individual in general, is coordinating with the autonomic nervous system of the other individual. And the insula is essentially splitting. One's attention between how we feel ourselves, how our body, feels what we're thinking with the thinking
1:08:54
and the body's bodily sensations of the other and that can be communicated. Obviously, through words.
1:09:00
Can be communicate through sounds. It can be communicated through
1:09:03
touch and it can be communicated through a number
1:09:05
of kind of more subtle cues like pupil size, or whether or not certainly in cases, where we recognize the person and we kind of know their responses, their autonomic responses under different conditions. We can assess it is the person comfortable, are they uncomfortable? Are they
1:09:21
are they more focused on me or on themselves? This is the coordinated silent dance. That if we look at a neurobiological terms, we can really see is all
1:09:30
About the autonomic nervous system whether or not it's time to tip the Seesaw to want it to one side or the other, depending on whether or not the other person seesaw is
1:09:40
tilted higher or lower than the other.
1:09:42
Okay. So we have the autonomic nervous system. And then we have this thing that we're calling empathy, which is really about
1:09:47
autonomic matching and again, the insula and the prefrontal
1:09:51
cortex are neural circuits that are crucial
1:09:54
for autonomic matching because they allow us to say what's out there and do I want to match to it or not.
1:10:00
Okay, and then the third category of neural circuit that Helen Fisher and others
1:10:05
have found to be important for desire love. And
1:10:07
attachment is the neural circuit associated with self-delusion. What do we mean by that? Well, first of all,
1:10:16
Self-delusion implies a kind of cynicism
1:10:20
about love and attachment and I
1:10:22
think it was George Bernard. Shaw that said love is really about
1:10:25
overestimating. The differences between individuals, actually when I hear
1:10:29
that and as I say it, I really don't like that quote. I have no bone to pick with George Bernard Shaw, but what it suggests
1:10:36
and I think what he meant was that, you know,
1:10:40
in love and attachment we tend to put so much value
1:10:43
in the other that we forget that many of the
1:10:46
Seas that are going on in our brain and body actually could be evoked by many other people too. But I
1:10:52
think it's somewhat overlooks.
1:10:54
The enormous power of attachment and the ways in which somebody smell somebody's voice, somebody's everything or
1:11:02
somebody's particular thing or things can really become so vital for our autonomic nervous
1:11:08
system to feel soothed and to feel elated etcetera.
1:11:11
So, I think that, while the quote is accurate, in the one sense,
1:11:16
I think it does overlook
1:11:18
the neural circuits for attachment and just How Deeply why are those can become for us? So I
1:11:24
will balance that quote with an
1:11:26
enormous number of other quotes that I won't mention. But that you can find out
1:11:30
there that really point to, you know, how incredible
1:11:34
the person is that one tends to be attached to that. There's really only one or, you know, or several people that could ever exist that could evoke those feelings from us, and, of course, you can read your, you know, naru.
1:11:46
Poetry in your that you can find these things all over the place in music and poetry and
1:11:50
writing. So for every cynical quote about these neural circuits, being generic
1:11:55
and could be activated by anybody. I think you'll find an ample number of
1:12:01
opposing quotes. That these neural circuits
1:12:03
can only be activated by that special someone or that particular person or maybe in just a small set of those
1:12:09
people. So what of delusion? Well, the work of Helen Fisher and others has really
1:12:16
Pointed to the fact that desire love and attachment are three separate
1:12:21
phases of what we call romantic relationships that typically, not always but typically desire tends to come first or falls in the early
1:12:30
phase that the process of
1:12:34
romantic, / sexual interactions. It doesn't necessarily have to be sex
1:12:38
itself, but certainly something that involves in some intimacy of some
1:12:43
kind, right? And generally Touch of some kind.
1:12:46
Eventually transitions into what we call love, which, eventually transitions into what we
1:12:51
call attachment. And I should just mention touch because touch is a fundamental aspect of this whole process. There's an article, a research article. I should say.
1:13:03
The title of it is relationship specific, encoding of social touch, and somatosensory and insular,
1:13:08
cortices cortices being cortex, cortex is plural, cortices. And again, there's our friend, the
1:13:15
insula. So this
1:13:16
A study that explored what brain areas and what body areas are activated by specific forms
1:13:23
of attachment and social touch.
1:13:25
And what they found not surprisingly, is that the, the, the areas of the brain are associated
1:13:30
with touch the somatosensory areas, but
1:13:32
more interestingly, the insular cortex are only
1:13:36
activated by touch.
1:13:38
So touch in the amount of touching and proximity and skin contact. Not
1:13:43
surprisingly activates brain areas associated with
1:13:46
Somatosensory touch, but also the insular cortex, which again, is this brain area, that
1:13:52
links, the internal our
1:13:53
feelings about what's going on inside Us in at the surface of our skin, with events
1:13:57
external. And they found activation of number of brain areas the amygdala orbitofrontal, cortex, and so on, and so on.
1:14:05
That's not as essential as just understanding that. The insula is the place in which we start to take our experience of our internal landscape,
1:14:13
attach that to our perceptions of the external landscape and then a sign that a
1:14:18
value or a sign that some sort of interpretation and positive delusion is predictive of long-term attachment. What do we mean by positive delusion? Positive delusion is the contradiction of that. George Bernard Shaw. Quote. It's
1:14:35
The belief that only this person can make me feel this way. This other person holds the
1:14:42
capacity to make me feel this way physically, or emotionally or both.
1:14:47
And so, as we move
1:14:49
from desire to love to attachment our brain, circuitry is a
1:14:54
literally getting tuned up such that that individual,
1:14:57
that we happen to be attached to again here thinking about monogamous relationships, but it, I guess for non-monogamous relationship be
1:15:05
Jules
1:15:06
is an are the way in
1:15:08
which our autonomic nervous system can be regulated. They actually
1:15:11
get access to our control panel. So it's our autonomic nervous system empathy and this
1:15:17
positive delusion.
1:15:19
Now, positive dilution is critical. If you look at the
1:15:23
stability of relationships over time something that's been extensively
1:15:27
studied mainly by psychologists, but now also by neurobiologists what you find is that there are some
1:15:35
Features of interactions between
1:15:37
individuals that predict that a relationship will last
1:15:41
and those are many, but mainly fall under this category of
1:15:45
positive delusions. I'll return to those in what those exactly look like.
1:15:48
But there are also just a
1:15:50
handful of things that predict that relationship will fail
1:15:54
over time. This is largely, the work of the gottman's is actually a husband and wife
1:15:59
team up at the University of Washington in Seattle.
1:16:03
The gottman's have
1:16:05
run a
1:16:05
laboratory in the department of psychology for a
1:16:07
long time. They've also done a lot of public
1:16:09
facing work around relationships,
1:16:10
and they've talked about the
1:16:12
various aspects of relationships and interactions between people that predict either staying together or breaking
1:16:20
up so much so that they've established a method by which they can look at video
1:16:25
interactions between couples and
1:16:28
with very high degree of certainty predict whether or not those
1:16:31
couples will stay together or break up over time and they've identified what they
1:16:35
All the Four Horsemen of relationships. These are things that essentially almost always predict that a couple will break up. And I think the current number on this is that gottman can predict divorce with 94% accuracy,
1:16:53
which if you think about is pretty remarkable, so, even though
1:16:55
these are purely psychological studies, I'm not
1:16:58
aware of any analysis of underlying
1:17:00
physiology. There are some things that they can observe between couples.
1:17:05
That can lead them to predict whether or not a couple of stay together or break up with
1:17:09
94% accuracy. So what are those things?
1:17:12
Those four behaviors? What they call the Four
1:17:14
Horsemen of the Apocalypse
1:17:17
for relationships or one criticism to defensiveness three stonewalling and for contempt with contempt, being the most powerful predictor of breaking up criticism, of course.
1:17:35
Not mean that there's no place for
1:17:38
criticism in stable, relationships. Of course, there
1:17:41
is has to do with how frequent and how intensely that criticism is voiced. Defensiveness, of course, is defensiveness. We know,
1:17:49
as the sort of lack of ability to hear another, or to adopt their stance. So lack of empathy, I think is a one way to interpret defensiveness
1:18:00
stonewalling, which is actually another form of
1:18:03
lack of empathy. It's a turning off of this.
1:18:05
Neural circuit. That's so critical for desire, love and
1:18:08
attachment. The stonewalling essentially means the
1:18:12
emotional response or the request of another is completely cut off. So it's, it's I don't think there's been brain Imaging of this, but we I think we can reasonably imagine that it
1:18:21
involves untethering, your insulin response from
1:18:25
the other and what they're dealing with and focusing, your insular response, huh? No pun,
1:18:30
intended on your own
1:18:31
internal state or perhaps the state of someone else in.
1:18:35
Early, we'll talk about infidelity in a moment
1:18:37
and then contempt and contempt has actually been referred to as the sulfuric
1:18:41
acid of relationship. I didn't say that, but gottman and colleagues have that. It is such a powerful predictor of divorce and breakups in the
1:18:50
future and contempt. Of course, by definition is the feeling that a person or thing is beneath consideration worthlessness or
1:18:58
deserving scorn.
1:19:00
And apparently, they can identify this in the videos
1:19:03
of couples having discussions in
1:19:05
Interacting by
1:19:07
very elaborate eye rolls by expressions of anger in one individual
1:19:12
when their partner is actually expressing enthusiasm or excitement about something. It's the oh, yeah, you would say that or we're deep-seated resentment toward the other so much
1:19:23
so that it's apparent. That
1:19:25
one kind of actively dislikes, the other
1:19:28
partner so contempt disregard for
1:19:33
something. That should be taken into account is the other way to the
1:19:35
About it, runs counter to all of the neural circuits, all three of the neural
1:19:40
circuits that we talked about before. It certainly is it is the antithesis of
1:19:44
empathy, it is anything, but a positive delusion.
1:19:47
It's really looking at the other
1:19:48
individual either accurately or inaccurately
1:19:51
as somebody that you kind of
1:19:52
despise. And then it is an absolute inversion of the autonomic
1:19:58
seesaw matching that I was talking about before it's a dissociating of
1:20:01
your seesaw from their seesaw. They're very excited about something.
1:20:05
Your unexcited by it. In fact, it's an inversion of their seesaw where
1:20:09
they're excited. You're down there down. You're up. Okay, so it's a basically, a, an inversion of all of the neural circuits that
1:20:20
Helen Fisher and others have identified as critical for desired love and attachment and therefore it's not surprising. That is so strongly predictive of breakups. And in the case of married, couples of divorce, for those of you that are interested in the work of the gottman's, and
1:20:35
Similar work. They've written several popular books. They're fairly easy to find. We can link to one of those in
1:20:42
the caption, but they've also developed some quite interesting online resources in their
1:20:48
so-called love lab. I guess it's fortunate that they didn't call it, the hate lab or The Break-Up lab, because they focused a lot on what predicts breakups,
1:20:56
but they've also written extensively,
1:20:59
and researched extensively in peer-reviewed studies. What makes people
1:21:05
People find appropriate partners for them and to maintain those Partnerships over time. So you can go, you can
1:21:14
search for love, lab
1:21:15
University of Washington gottman, or any number of their various books. I think you'll find some some useful resources there.
1:21:24
I want to shift back to
1:21:25
the work of Helen Fisher. She's
1:21:28
made some very interesting statements and some
1:21:31
very interesting observations that at least to my
1:21:34
mind map very well on to the
1:21:36
knowledge of neural circuitry both in humans and non-human primates and in other species. I realized that she's the only name in the game, but she's made some
1:21:47
observations that I think are are very, as we say parsimonious. Meaning, they allow us to organize a lot
1:21:53
of this stuff in
1:21:53
You some distinct Frameworks. She's also done. Some really beautiful studies that involve data from millions or even tens of millions of individuals on dating site. So I'm going to share those with you in a moment. But before I
1:22:06
do that, I just want to
1:22:09
paraphrase. Dr. Fisher,
1:22:11
who said that? Sex
1:22:14
drive or desire? That the pursuit of someone to mate with meaning to mate the verb, not necessarily to find a mate.
1:22:23
May be she didn't say definitively but maybe a way to
1:22:27
forage for a potential love partners that the The Arc
1:22:30
of this whole business is really the order that we're describing it, that it's desire,
1:22:36
then love, and
1:22:38
then attachment
1:22:39
and that often times, people can get a
1:22:41
confused. You may know some of these people you may be one of these individuals who might confuse desire for attachment or might
1:22:50
confuse love
1:22:52
for attachment.
1:22:53
That there's a
1:22:54
sequence of recruitment of these neural
1:22:57
circuits that's established. First from the
1:22:59
pursuit of someone to mate with and
1:23:03
she's places in the context of kind of more modern dating themes. Where depending on culture, people might explore
1:23:12
several may be many many individuals before quote-unquote settling down with with somebody at least for some period of time.
1:23:21
I think that's an interesting framework because
1:23:23
Was it circumvents a lot of the frankly unanswerable questions about whether or not you know,
1:23:31
humans were meant to be monogamous or whether or not they
1:23:33
weren't those are conversations that hold cultural context, that hold all
1:23:38
sorts of. Contexts, that really can't be addressed in a laboratory setting,
1:23:42
but this idea that sex drive is a way to forage for potential, love partners. And that love is a kind of a litmus test for whether or not
1:23:51
longer or shorter more.
1:23:53
Attachments can and will form is one that at least makes sense to me later in the episode. We'll talk
1:23:59
about this notion of sex drive and desire. I'll actually talk about some tools that are that have very strong data, really, to support them in terms of things that people can do or take to increase libido, both men and women, because our, there's actually quite good data on that
1:24:16
now, but in the meantime, I want to talk about some of the work that dr. Fisher has done in terms of categorizing people.
1:24:23
People into again. We have four groups. These are distinct
1:24:28
from the ABC and D attachment Styles described earlier,
1:24:32
although as I described them. You might be
1:24:34
able to map them somewhat onto those.
1:24:36
And these four groups are groups that were defined through her studies of people that were or
1:24:44
are, I don't know if they were or if they are still on match.com,
1:24:49
but a very extensive data set. So again, Millions if not tens of
1:24:53
Millions of individuals, the number I heard her quote and I forgive me if this is not accurate is that in upwards of 40 million individuals?
1:25:04
In terms of whether or not they're neurochemical and hormone systems are tuned toward particular types of behaviors. What do I mean by that? Well, both men and women males and females have both testosterone and estrogen. Typically
1:25:20
again, these are
1:25:21
averages but typically men have more testosterone than they do estrogen and they have more testosterone than do women and less estrogen than do women. Typically women have more estrogen than they do testosterone again averages.
1:25:34
And they have less
1:25:36
testosterone than men more estrogen, the men, and so, and so on and so forth,
1:25:40
but both hormone systems are active in
1:25:43
both sets of
1:25:45
individuals. And, of course, all humans. As far as we know manufacturer, both dopamine and serotonin dopamine, as I mentioned earlier, has a number of effects in the brain and body, but one of the primary effects is that it
1:25:58
places us into states of motivation and pursuit for
1:26:02
various things. There is a
1:26:04
Somewhat, close relationship between the dopamine system and the testosterone system in the hypothalamus,
1:26:11
this brain area above the roof of your mouth, and the pituitary
1:26:14
gland, which is responsible for making
1:26:16
hormones, that make the ovaries and, or testes
1:26:19
secrete testosterone or estrogen. So, there's a lot of signaling that occurs such that dopamine in testosterone tend to operate as kind of close cousins in a system of pursuit and conversely.
1:26:34
The serotonin system tends to on average collaborate with the estrogen system to impart certain
1:26:41
physiological functions and behaviors. So these aren't hard and fast or I guess better stated. These aren't
1:26:49
strict, black and white categorizations, but I think those General themes hold. When
1:26:53
you look at the animal and human data, so
1:26:57
dr. Fisher has taken some some Liberties, but I think they are what I would call logically in.
1:27:04
Glee in neurobiologically grounded, Liberties in classifying individuals who are on these dating sites. According to the types of things, they report about themselves. And the type of people they tend to match with on these dating sites and created these four categories categories. The four categories, are she calls one, the dopamine
1:27:26
category. So these are people
1:27:28
who would have high dopamine. And again,
1:27:32
that's just a name.
1:27:34
Doesn't mean they have
1:27:35
low anything else but they are high on the dopamine scale, people who rate high on on the dopamine scale, tend to be
1:27:43
what the scientists and psychologists call High sensation seeking novel, seeking. They like new things. They like spontaneity. They tend to be very adventurous
1:27:51
and I think that's largely true. If you look at it conditions where dopamine is super physiological. It's elevated Beyond. I'm normal
1:28:01
levels. Things like Mania.
1:28:04
Yeah, or when people take certain drugs of abuse
1:28:07
like cocaine or amphetamine, that really raised dopamine levels up very, very high for some period of time. They do tend to increase energy energy
1:28:16
motivation and Novelty seeking, and of course, drugs, like amphetamine and cocaine have all sorts of deleterious effects that I don't need to go into here, but it's worth pointing
1:28:24
out, but they don't tend to make people
1:28:26
calm and relaxed and seek. Soothing
1:28:29
interactions. Conversely the group that dr. Fisher called.
1:28:34
The serotonin group
1:28:36
tend to be more grounded. In soothing activities, quiescent type activities. They actually tend to be on average. They tend to like rules and follow rules. They tend to be
1:28:48
homebodies. This sort of thing. They're really. You can imagine them the sort of
1:28:52
stable types, but they really
1:28:54
like stability. They're not really into spontaneity as much again,
1:28:57
averages, and then she created two other categories, the testosterone category of high test.
1:29:04
Roster own this again could be males or females and then the estrogen
1:29:10
category again could be males or
1:29:11
females and she gave these different names that I won't go into here. You can look up her work
1:29:16
online, but she, you know, the names like the director and and the follower and things like
1:29:20
that, but I don't really want to use those as much as I want to stick to the biological terms. So we have dopamine serotonin testosterone and estrogen. Now that might seem like an unfair kind of over generalization, but
1:29:34
What's interesting is not necessarily the name or the neurochemical system, right? Those could have just been called category, 1, 2, 3 and 4 for all
1:29:42
that that
1:29:44
matters here. What is interesting is seeing how those different groups of individuals that she absolutely can categorize based on their self-reported
1:29:56
preferences about behaviors and certain kinds of interactions
1:30:00
how those groups tend to pair up with people in the
1:30:03
same.
1:30:04
Or opposite categories. So what her studies
1:30:06
reveal is that people that fall into the high sensation
1:30:09
seeking novelty, seeking spontaneity category, the one that she calls the high dopamine category tend to
1:30:16
pair up with at least in the short-term tend to pair up with people who are also in that dopaminergic category. So these would be people that would spontaneously take a trip or explore something new or new restaurant. They tend to be creative and explorative
1:30:33
types.
1:30:34
So that group, on average tends to date and mate and potentially form long-term relationships within
1:30:43
category, again, averages
1:30:46
individuals that she placed into the serotonin group or
1:30:50
the what she hypothesize would be a high serotonin group. Can. They didn't measure serotonin.
1:30:54
But people that tend to place value on stability on rules on certain forms of kind of traditional organization.
1:31:04
At home. And in relationships, those people also tended to pair up with selectdate.
1:31:11
We presume mate with and form stable relationships with people in the same category.
1:31:17
Now, individuals in the other two categories, the high testosterone group and again, testosterone wasn't measured, but she called it the high testosterone group, but these are people that tend to be very directive. They tend to know what they want and are comfortable telling other people.
1:31:34
What they want and from them, these are individuals that in her studies and in other studies tend to be a little bit challenging meaning, they not necessarily challenging to be around, but they tend to challenge other people kind of push them in order to expand their boundaries either for sake of the relationship or just in general. And the people, they tend to push are the people that they pair up with, which are the people in
1:31:59
the estrogen category, what she called. High
1:32:01
estrogen again, they didn't measure estrogen.
1:32:04
But the people in the estrogen category, where the ones that describe themselves and their choices in life and their preferences as being nurturing. They actually seem to like
1:32:14
it. When someone else is making the major decisions, not every decision. They
1:32:19
certainly like to be heard, of course, in terms of their preferences, but that those two types the what she called the
1:32:26
testosterone and the estrogen type tend to pair
1:32:28
up. So why are these
1:32:30
categorizations and these averages? Interesting to me, at least?
1:32:33
Interesting enough to convey to you.
1:32:35
The reason they're interesting to me is again, not because of their names. These molecules were not measured in these individuals, but that they once again, bring us to the themes that we addressed
1:32:47
before
1:32:47
which are the autonomic nervous system and whether or not it tends to be shifted more towards alertness in action, or more towards kind of a stable equilibrium or more towards kind of calm and whether or not individuals are selecting for
1:33:04
Or people who have autonomic nervous systems that are more or less, like there's before they even meet right. So again going back to this seesaw an allergy. It's almost like people who have the kind of flat seesaw alert, but calm but not extremely alert, not extremely overly calm and in situations, but kind of in the middle seem to be seeking out
1:33:26
people that are also at that kind of autonomic equilibrium people in the what she called, the
1:33:32
dopamine category, which
1:33:34
Really can just be described as high sensation seeking novelty seeking. They seem to want to pair with one another. So there's a selection for similar in two of the group's autonomic tone. I find that very interesting because in that decision or that preference for similar autonomic Tone. It essentially eliminates a lot of the, the requirement for figuring out how to match one's autonomic nervous system to another. They
1:34:03
They simply find someone with a similar tendency. Okay, whereas in the other two groups that she called testosterone and estrogen, the director type and the nurturing kind of somewhat follower type. There's a an establishment of balance, but not in the in between two individuals as a match. But rather on the whole in the relationship, one person is kind of
1:34:27
driving the novelty seeking and the course of decisions and actions. And the other person is
1:34:32
essentially agreeing to those
1:34:34
Assuming that those decisions are good for both people
1:34:37
and I emphasized good for both people because one of the themes that dr. Fisher underscores than I'd like to underscore here as well. Is that
1:34:46
It need not be the case that
1:34:47
people pair up. Exactly. According to these categorizations that I've described dopamine with dopamine serotonin, with serotonin testosterone with estrogen. So on
1:34:57
what is important is that there be a recognition and a respect for the other types or a recognition in a respect for the fact that both are of the same type. You could actually imagine for instance that to people of this
1:35:11
High sensation seeking novelty seeking could have a
1:35:14
terrifically
1:35:15
In relationship, but that it actually might be a relationship in with, in which the financial stability isn't quite there or in which the basic stability isn't there.
1:35:27
You could imagine, for instance, a situation in, which a relationship between two people of what she called, the high serotonin preference would have a relationship that was actually kind of dull in which, both of them found themselves,
1:35:41
kind of bored at some point or in which, there wasn't enough of die.
1:35:45
Of the dynamic tension that sometimes is required in order to keep this cycle of Desire love and attachment going something that we will talk about in a
1:35:54
moment. So the point here is not that one should necessarily pair up according to
1:36:00
the these Arrangements that I
1:36:02
described. The point is that on average
1:36:04
that's what tends to happen
1:36:06
and that through a recognition that these categorizations
1:36:10
exist similar to the recognition that the
1:36:15
Type ABC and D infant, and toddler type of attachments exists that we can gain better. Self awareness of
1:36:23
who we are and how we tend to show up in romantic attachments
1:36:26
and thereby navigate healthier, mate, seeking healthier breakups, if the case dictates it, and in some cases, healthy long-term relationships by understanding that the other person can either be similar or complementary to us.
1:36:44
One is neither better than the other it simply the case that in all
1:36:49
romantic attachments
1:36:50
from the initial Inception
1:36:53
of the Romantic attachment desire.
1:36:56
Love and attachment. There is an autonomic coordination.
1:37:01
And of course, there is coordination of all sorts of other things, like, you know, food sex, and sleep, and finances and where people are going to live and many other features. But that at the
1:37:11
core of all, that is a seeking of
1:37:14
Either autonomic likeness or autonomic differences. And I think that recognition can
1:37:21
be extremely valuable in thinking about
1:37:23
tools to enter and maintain relationships. If one thinks about their autonomic nervous system, not simply as something that is driven by external
1:37:33
people and events, but that we can actually gain some control over through
1:37:38
techniques of the sort that I talked about earlier and on previous podcast, but also generally, if we are able to
1:37:44
Just our autonomic nervous system in order
1:37:47
to at least appreciate or get some empathy into what someone else is experiencing. Then we gain actual
1:37:54
cognitive empathy
1:37:56
and this episode isn't about empathy per se, but the theme keeps coming up again and again and I think it's worth mentioning that when you talk to
1:38:04
psychologists whether or not they're psycho analyst or from another source of training, what you
1:38:10
find is that they don't talk about empathy as a general term. They will talk about
1:38:15
Emotional empathy, they'll talk about cognitive empathy and what I'm talking
1:38:19
about here today, is that yet a third category that is very strongly determinant of relationship Dynamics. And that's autonomic empathy. I'm a biologist. I'm not a psychologist. So I love mechanism and
1:38:33
fortunately. There are studies that have been done recently using modern techniques to look at neural mechanisms of
1:38:40
romantic attachment.
1:38:41
I mentioned earlier, some of the brain
1:38:43
Imaging studies.
1:38:44
That have been done on child and
1:38:47
mother literally Imaging the activity of neurons in the
1:38:50
brain as child is
1:38:52
nursing or as a mother is
1:38:54
soothing baby. And as you learned earlier, baby is soothing mother as well. Those are remarkable studies. You may have seen some of these pictures online. You can see the kind of silhouette of the infant and mother and their brains and evens on the brain. Activation patterns, really, really beautiful
1:39:08
studies. Similar Studies have been done in romantic couples with those couples.
1:39:14
Has either touching one another touching and kissing or in kind of clever, I think control experiments of the
1:39:22
person just touching a pillow or something or kissing a pillow in order to try and create the most reasonable control for what are actually pretty complicated interpersonal Dynamics to do in a brain Imaging scanner.
1:39:36
But some of the other studies that have been done recently involved so-called EEG. So these are electrical recordings that are done non-invasively.
1:39:44
Putting a bunch of electrodes on the outside of the
1:39:46
scalp. EEG is useful. In that, you can
1:39:51
do it non-invasively. You can do it in while people are
1:39:55
moving and doing things kissing touching Etc. It doesn't allow one to image or to evaluate
1:40:02
neural activity. Very deeply in the brain. So you
1:40:04
can miss out on a lot of things. It's sort of like looking at the wave
1:40:07
structure on the ocean without actually looking into the depths of the, of the ocean so you can miss certain things, but if you see things,
1:40:14
So generally you trust, they are there but you can't see what you don't see
1:40:18
nonetheless. There are some studies that I'll just point you to and that form the segue for what I'm going to discuss in a moment, which is a study published in scientific reports in 2021 entitled investigating, real life, emotions and romantic couples a mobile
1:40:37
EEG study. So this is as the
1:40:40
title suggests, I think people wear these EEG caps of electrodes.
1:40:44
Get engaged. In very
1:40:46
passionate emotional. Kisses emotional speech toward one, another standing at different distances. So a lot of cool stuff that you can do that. You
1:40:53
really couldn't do in a brain scanner because in a brain scanner people have to be there and usually in a b bar there actually to draw for extra like this. I've been in one of these things. There's not a lot of moving around to be had at least not using the current technology.
1:41:07
In any case, what they found was there is a shift in brain waves, brain
1:41:12
States things.
1:41:14
Like Alpha Waves was just a particular frequency of brain waves in the neocortex, the kind of outer shell of the brain, just beneath the skull,
1:41:22
and in people that are kissing or in people, they're engaged in romantic speech.
1:41:27
Or I didn't actually hear what they said to one another, but what the couple seems exciting romantic and
1:41:35
arousing to them. They see more Alpha wave activity. Compared to the control conditions and there was some what we
1:41:41
call lateralization where the left hemisphere was more active than the
1:41:44
And so forth. And
1:41:47
these studies are important because we know that the autonomic nervous
1:41:51
systems of individuals tend to
1:41:53
start to collaborate and actually
1:41:56
synchronize at the level of heart beats at the level of breathing during romantic interactions of different
1:42:02
kinds. But these studies are some of the first of their kind to start looking at neural
1:42:07
synchronization, between
1:42:08
individuals. Now, the simple version of looking at this
1:42:14
and the
1:42:14
The way I would have got this would all go was
1:42:17
okay to people start kissing. They start talking about what
1:42:20
they find particularly romantic and
1:42:21
arousing for them and their brain waves will just
1:42:23
match to one another, and that's really the basis of romantic attachment and romantic engagement in that sort of thing,
1:42:32
but it turns out that the opposite is true. So a really nice study. Publishing it in a really fine Journal cerebral. Cortex
1:42:41
is a journal that I've known about for many years. They publish strong and
1:42:44
Me physiology in neuroimaging. There's a study that was published first author, kajiura in and this paper really points. Again. This is 2021. And the title of this paper is brain knows who is on the same wavelength resting state connectivity can predict
1:43:03
compatibility of female, male relationship.
1:43:06
Now, what this study did was a little bit different, they looked at the resting or default mode activity of the brain. So rather than
1:43:14
Poked activity, as it's called where people are kissing or engage in some sort of activity.
1:43:19
This was neuroimaging study. Not EEG,
1:43:23
but fmri functional magnetic resonance. Imaging which is similar
1:43:27
to EG in principle, but allows you to look
1:43:29
deep into the brain and it has very good resolution in time and space. So fast events can be monitored and the precise location of those events can be monitored somewhat better than EG. There are exceptions to
1:43:44
So for you, EG years out there EEG,
1:43:47
don't don't come
1:43:48
after me with electrodes. Just understand that fmri
1:43:52
gives you a fuller picture of what's going on. And what cut uemura at all found was that contrary to what your reflexive
1:44:02
prediction might be
1:44:05
people tend to select people that have resting brain states that are different than theirs or
1:44:12
sometimes they found that are actually
1:44:14
The opposite to their own resting brain
1:44:16
State. And you might say, well that doesn't make any sense. I thought this is all about autonomic coordination. But actually, if we go back
1:44:23
to hell and fissures categorizations of the dopamine types, the sensation-seeking types that is
1:44:29
serotonin, the kind
1:44:30
of stable rule
1:44:32
following types testosterone and estrogen types. Remember that the two
1:44:35
categories that she called testosterone, estrogen type the director, and the, and the follower the nurturer, I guess that would be the more accurate way, the director and the
1:44:42
nurture. Those
1:44:44
A pair up across
1:44:46
categories, not within
1:44:47
category. And so, I think what's really needed for this field, which to
1:44:51
my knowledge hasn't happened yet is
1:44:54
to really start to map the neuroanatomical and
1:44:58
neurophysiological findings of, in this case that resting brain state is
1:45:04
in one form in one
1:45:06
individual. And they tend to seek out people whose resting brain state is different than theirs. Not similar.
1:45:11
That needs to be mapped onto the Moor.
1:45:14
Or subjective psychological
1:45:17
categorizations, that Helen Fisher and indeed the gottman's and others have
1:45:21
created the sort of the state of the field. Now and I mention this not to confuse you but to the contrary to illustrate, that it's not
1:45:29
just about finding someone at just like you. And it's not just about finding someone whose opposite to you. This is actually the reason that I decided to become a biologist at some point in my life,
1:45:40
which is that we can find verbal sayings and stories and examples.
1:45:44
To support just about anything. It's this is not a knock on the field of psychology as you can probably tell from today's episode. I have great respect for in reverence for the field of psychology, especially its collaboration with neuroscience and vice versa.
1:45:58
But in the popular culture, we can find examples and sayings that support it essentially,
1:46:04
anything as it relates to a
1:46:05
relationship. For instance. I've heard and you've probably heard absence makes the heart grow fonder. And indeed I've experienced that and I believe it's
1:46:12
true, but I also
1:46:14
So have experienced and I believe to be true that out of sight out of mind, also exist in that there will be a biological mechanism for that. The
1:46:21
point here is that matching of same to same or same two different can both
1:46:26
be effective in creating the
1:46:28
desire. Love attachment
1:46:30
process.
1:46:31
It's a matter of who is looking for same, and who is looking for different and there, I think dr. Fisher and the work of these neurophysiologists and brain. Imagers, really does point in a Direction.
1:46:44
Whereby there is not one form of
1:46:47
attachment that is going to be holy Above All Else and will predict good outcomes.
1:46:53
There is not going to be a case in
1:46:55
which Opposites Attract and that's always the best rule to follow. Sometimes it will sometimes it
1:47:00
won't. There is also not the case that people tend to pair up with similar. Sometimes it will be the case. Sometimes it won't. Now there are
1:47:09
certain statistics that support that statement. For
1:47:11
instance, people on average people pair up.
1:47:14
Up with individuals of
1:47:16
similar educational background income and attractiveness that is true on average but it's not always the case.
1:47:22
And again, a knowledge of
1:47:24
and a respect for the different categorizations of attachment, the
1:47:27
different
1:47:28
categorizations of mate seeking described by Fisher and
1:47:31
others. And the recognition that matching of autonomic nervous systems, but also mismatching of resting state. Brain networks are all at
1:47:39
play in driving what we are calling desire, love and
1:47:42
attachment. So in keeping with the
1:47:44
Duration of the fact that there's a saying or a book, or a song or an example of pretty much any
1:47:50
relationship. Dynamic,
1:47:52
I wanted now talk about an article that came out a little over 10 years ago, that talked about the
1:47:59
universality of love and the ability to fall in
1:48:02
love. So this would be very much in line with the
1:48:04
George Bernard. Shaw? Quote, that I mentioned earlier that love is really overestimating, the differences between individuals.
1:48:10
And again, I should say that is
1:48:11
not something that I personally believe, although, maybe I'm
1:48:14
Just deluding myself. I like to think that the people that we fall in love with our really special for us that they could not easily be replaced with anybody else that simply my stance. I'm not basing that on any hardcore neurobiological mechanism, but nonetheless
1:48:34
an article was published in the New York Times in 2015, that related to some psychological studies that were done as well as some clinical work as well as some
1:48:44
I would, I would call pop psychology or things that fall outside that the domains of academic science.
1:48:50
And the whole basis of this article was 36 questions that lead to love. And it involved a listing
1:49:00
out. Indeed of 36 questions, set
1:49:03
divided into set 1, set 2 and set three that progress from somewhat ordinary questions about life experience and self-reports two more.
1:49:14
Let's call them deep questions about
1:49:16
people's values and things that are emotionally close to them. And I'll just give an example of a few of these. You can find this easily online by just putting into the your search engine 36 questions that lead to love some of the questions in set. Number one were for instance. What would constitute a perfect day for you? For what in your life. Do you feel most grateful kind of standard questionnaire
1:49:38
stuff in set to what is your most treasured memory was your most terrible memory. So these are
1:49:44
Until our drilling a little bit deeper, into one's personal experience, and emotional system.
1:49:49
And then set three questions. 25 through 36 are things, you know, what is a very embarrassing moment in your
1:49:59
life. When did you last cry in front of another person? And by yourself?
1:50:04
What is something that's too serious to be joked about. So it's going deeper into one's emotional system and even questions, like, of all the people in your family,
1:50:13
who's death?
1:50:14
Would you find most disturbing and why so pretty, pretty heavy stuff there at the end. Now, the
1:50:19
reason this article got so much traction in. The reason I'm bringing up today is that there was a statement that was made in and around this article that if two people
1:50:30
Went on a date
1:50:32
or simply sat down and asked each other. These questions and each answer these questions, and
1:50:39
the other
1:50:41
was paying attention carefully and
1:50:44
at some level emotionally responding or not
1:50:47
responding, but certainly paying attention to the answers of the other
1:50:50
person that by the end of that exchange where one person asks,
1:50:55
36, questions in the other person answers all 36. And then the other person,
1:51:00
All 36 in the other person answers, all 36 that they would fall in love, right? Which seems like a kind of a ridiculous thing.
1:51:08
And yet it is the case that people who go through this exercise report feeling as if they know the other person, quite well and feeling certain levels of attachment or even love and desire for the other person that they were would not have predicted. Scuse me would not have predicted. Had they not gone through.
1:51:30
That
1:51:30
process. So, what's going on in this exchange of questions and answers of a progressively more emotional and deep level.
1:51:38
Well, what I predict is going on, is that inside of that exchange people are creating a sort of delusional story about the
1:51:46
nature of the exchange, being a reflection of some deeper
1:51:50
attachment. And so even though people are just exchanging words. They're not physically touching.
1:51:56
They are not at least, not at the point where there?
1:52:00
These kinds of questionnaire studies, they may touch afterwards for all I know and probably did in some cases
1:52:07
but they're not exchanging life experience in
1:52:10
an immediate way. They're not actually going off into the world and doing things together yet.
1:52:15
They are simply exchanging narrative, but we know, based on recent studies, and I've covered this before on
1:52:22
this podcast, but I'll mention again. There was
1:52:24
a study published in cell reports, a cell, Press Journal, excellent Journal showing that.
1:52:30
That when individuals listen to the same narrative, their heart rates tend to
1:52:34
synchronize or at least follow a very
1:52:36
similar pattern, even if they're not in the same room
1:52:39
listening to a given narrative,
1:52:41
whereas in this case people are facing one another listening to the narratives of each other. Certainly, they are having autonomic responses. And it stands to reason that their autonomic nervous systems are synchronizing much in the same way that the cell report study found that people will synchronize their autonomic nervous systems to a
1:53:00
Shared heard story from another. In other
1:53:04
words, whether or not we hear a story, watch a movie, listen to a song or exchange our own individual stories. Our autonomic nervous systems have the potential
1:53:13
to map onto one another.
1:53:15
So I'm not all that surprised that people find that they fall in
1:53:19
love in quotes
1:53:21
after answering these questions to one another. Because essentially the way these questions are laid out, is they establish a narrative. They established a very
1:53:30
The Narrative and the other person is listening, very closely and we don't have physiological
1:53:34
or brain Imaging studies, to,
1:53:37
to support what I'm about to say, but the the reasonable
1:53:40
interpretation is that that's causing some sort of autonomic
1:53:44
synchronization. So, if you want to try this on a date, or even
1:53:48
it's actually been hypothesized. That this could be useful for existing couples,
1:53:53
even if they already know the answers to some of these questions. And that doesn't surprise me either. I think the autonomic coordination is present during
1:54:01
Mating behavior. Its present during shared experience of the outside world movies,
1:54:06
concerts watching. One's children with somebody else
1:54:09
etcetera and it's established by
1:54:11
sharing one's own Narrative of their own personal experience.
1:54:15
So I don't want to seem overly reductionist. I'll never proposed
1:54:19
that all of our sensation, perception action and experience in life, boils down to us, just being bags of chemicals and the action of those chemicals or any aspect of our nervous system. And yet,
1:54:30
Get
1:54:31
in looking across the psychological literature of
1:54:34
development of attachment, in the psychological literature of adult and romantic attachment and what makes and breaks. Those attachments. It's very clear to me and I
1:54:45
think courses through the literature at multiple levels that autonomic coordination is absolutely
1:54:51
key for the establishment of Desire love and attachment. In fact, I talked earlier about how our actual conception is born out of autonomic.
1:55:00
Even of one sort or another.
1:55:02
So again, it doesn't necessarily mean the autonomic nervous system is
1:55:07
always be synchronized. In the case of the to categorizations that fisher, proposed of the director / testosterone, type in the, the nurturing
1:55:17
follower / estrogen type. It was actually the coordination. But in opposite directions
1:55:24
of individuals that fall into each of those categories that led to more stable attachments, or the seeking out of those,
1:55:30
Attachments, that you would say.
1:55:31
But nonetheless, it's at least to my mind, very clear that autonomic coordination is a Hallmark feature of desire,
1:55:41
a Hallmark feature of what we call
1:55:43
love and a Hallmark feature
1:55:45
of what we call attachment and that the breaking of attachments or the failure has of Desire, the failures of love and the failures of attachment overtime in line with the work of gottman and others and in even just simply what's
1:56:00
Wired for mating behavior
1:56:02
is also
1:56:03
reflected in the autonomic nervous system. But in that case, a failure to coordinate the autonomic nervous systems in some sort of concerted way any
1:56:11
discussion about desire
1:56:12
love and attachment would be incomplete if we didn't talk about the dreaded infidelity and cheating. Much has been made of infidelity and cheating and whether or not people who are higher on dopamine and sensation-seeking tend to cheat more or less.
1:56:27
Frankly. I don't think there's any solid evidence for that.
1:56:30
That
1:56:31
I think there are a lot of examples that we can draw from in our own lives and in the lives of others, that would generally support one or the other model, but I'm not aware
1:56:40
of any decent
1:56:42
physiological studies or psychological studies. That really point to that, for instance. I
1:56:47
would never say that
1:56:49
the serotonergic phenotype as described by Fisher is less prone to cheat or that the, you know, people who have an insecure attachment or more likely to cheat. It says,
1:57:00
For instance. I don't think those correlations have been drawn in any kind of meaningful way yet. So I would be cautious about assigning them without that evidence. However, there are some
1:57:13
interesting studies involving again, neuroimaging
1:57:16
and some subjective measures in humans. Meaning asking them questions that there are good ways to tease out, lies from truths and in these sorts of studies,
1:57:25
and whether or not people tend to find their partner.
1:57:30
Or others more or less attractive, depending on how
1:57:34
people feel about themselves.
1:57:36
And I think this is a very
1:57:38
interesting aspect to desire, love and attachment. For the following
1:57:41
reason.
1:57:43
You hear a lot out there that, you know, in
1:57:46
order to form a really strong
1:57:48
relationship. You have to have a good relationship with yourself or you have to love yourself or you often hear for instance, that you know, it's exactly when you're not looking for a relationship that you're going to find what you hear this stuff. Right?
1:58:03
But none of that is really grounded in any studies. Again, that's like out of sight,
1:58:07
out of mind or absence makes the heart
1:58:08
grow fonder. There are many life examples to support those.
1:58:13
Statements and their many
1:58:14
life examples to support statements to the
1:58:16
opposite.
1:58:19
There's a particular study that I found. This was published in
1:58:22
Frontiers in Psychology, but it's a
1:58:24
experimental study, that involves neuroimaging the title. This study is manipulation of self-expansion Alters
1:58:32
responses to attractive alternative partners,
1:58:35
and I love the design of the study. What they did in this study is they took couples and they evaluated members of that relationship for what's called self-expansion now self-expansion.
1:58:48
Metric that involves one's perception of self as seen through
1:58:55
the relationship to the other. And this is something that was developed by the
1:59:00
authors are Aaron and
1:59:01
Aaron, so they have the same last name. So, I'm assuming this was either a sibling team or somehow related team were romantic couple team Aro, n and Aro, n
1:59:13
Erin and Aaron in 1986 proposed, the self-expansion
1:59:17
model of close.
1:59:18
And
1:59:18
chips and they propose that people are motivated to enter relationships. I'm reading here in order to enhance the
1:59:23
self and increase. Self-efficacy.
1:59:25
In other words that one of the
1:59:27
reasons why many people enter relationships
1:59:29
is that it makes us feel good about ourselves and more capable. And I
1:59:32
would see that as a healthy interdependence, not necessarily codependence.
1:59:38
This is especially strong at the beginning
1:59:40
of a relationship. It turns out
1:59:41
when people are forming pair bonds, and it's the case that pleasure arousal and
1:59:48
Again, all Hallmark features of autonomic nervous system function. Pleasure arousal and excitement. Give rise to self-expansion meaning to self-efficacy. So what this self-expansion model is really about is how great other people that we are close to in
2:00:05
romantically attached, you can potentially make us feel
2:00:09
in terms of what they say. In terms of what they do, in terms of the way in which we believe they feel about us
2:00:18
So it doesn't necessarily have to involve explicit
2:00:20
statements of them telling us how great we are or them doing great gestures for us. But how we actually feel, they feel about us
2:00:30
turns out to be a very strong parameter in terms of how we
2:00:33
feel about ourselves and the relationship
2:00:35
overall. Now, some of you out there are probably thinking oh, yeah isn't there this thing up the Love
2:00:39
Languages, right? I don't have any Neuroscience to support that. I think the Love Languages. I'm not super familiar with this. I didn't list it out, but that some people are
2:00:48
Yeah, their autonomic nervous system. If you will tend to be very responsive to gifts or to Quality Time or to physical touch or acts of kindness. I think I've got a few of these, right? I probably have a few wrong. Anyway, they're easy to find online.
2:01:01
And people do tend to have a kind of a biased
2:01:04
toward two or three of these things that are, especially meaningful for them. And when I hear meaningful, I hear they tend to push the autonomic nervous system and neurochemical systems of the brain and body in a direction that makes us feel good as opposed to lousy or neutral.
2:01:18
In any event, this study looked at whether or not people have high levels of self
2:01:25
expansion through the actions or statements of their significant
2:01:29
other and how that influences, their perception of people outside the relationship, meaning, how
2:01:37
attractive they perceive people outside, the relationship, to be
2:01:40
turns out to be strongly influenced by a, whether or not they're self-expansion.
2:01:49
Very strongly driven by the other person that they are involved with that. They're in the Romantic relationship with and whether or not that's being
2:01:56
expressed to them. So, here's how the study went.
2:01:59
First of all, they rated or
2:02:01
categorized individuals, on the basis of the self-expansion metric. Some people have more of a
2:02:07
potential to experience self-expansion through others. Write, some of us feel great about ourselves and we're kind of topped off at the others, don't feel so great about themselves, but
2:02:18
But they can feel much better in response to praise and particular, praise or self-expansion type behaviors, are statements from people that we really care about, and still other people
2:02:27
are a mixture of the two, the kind of moderate levels of both.
2:02:30
So they rated them on the scale and then they had people experience, self-expansion
2:02:36
narratives. They heard their significant other say, really terrific
2:02:39
things about them and about the relationship in particular, that the
2:02:43
relationship that they have was
2:02:45
exciting novel and
2:02:46
challenging. So that was one
2:02:48
Myself expansion and they went into some detail as to why that was the case in their particular
2:02:52
relationship. Or they heard a narrative from us from their significant other about
2:02:58
strong feelings of love between the two that had been experienced previously in the
2:03:04
relationship. So, in the one case, it sort of directed more towards them. And in the other case,
2:03:08
it's more about the relationship itself.
2:03:10
And then they did brain Imaging of one
2:03:13
person in the relationship. While that person assess the attractiveness of
2:03:18
S of people outside the relationship
2:03:21
and what they found was that people who were primed for this self-expansion had lower activation of brain areas associated with assessing others, attractiveness, then did the people
2:03:33
who experienced a lot of self-expansion.
2:03:36
Now, the takeaway from
2:03:38
that, at least the way I read this study is
2:03:41
if you're with somebody who really benefits from or experiences a lot of self expansion.
2:03:48
Unless you really want them
2:03:49
to pay attention to the attractiveness of other people. It
2:03:54
stands to reason that they would benefit from more
2:03:57
self-expansion type gestures or statements. Okay,
2:04:01
not so much centered on the
2:04:02
relationship. We have such a great relationship. There's so much love. It's so great
2:04:06
that too, but in the context of this study in these findings that the person is really terrific, that the relationship that they've created together, is
2:04:14
really exciting novel
2:04:15
and challenging that there's a narrative around the
2:04:18
Ship that really has a lot to do with the Dynamics between the individuals in particular, that the person who really likes self-expansion is vital to that Dynamic. Okay, so it's not looking down at the relationship as a set of equals. There is sort of this bias written into this of that. This person is really essential for the relationship. I'm not saying this is something that anyone has to do. I'm not saying this is right or wrong. This is just what the data say,
2:04:40
but what's remarkable is that, in the absence of those statements?
2:04:45
People who have or that rate high on this scale of self expansion, rate attractive, alternative Partners as more attractive. Now, that's interesting to me because it means that their actual perception
2:04:59
of others is changing.
2:05:00
It's not that their opportunity to
2:05:03
see others is changing, right? This is not a matter of them.
2:05:06
Somehow, getting access or no access
2:05:09
to attractive alternative Partners again, attractive alternative Partners, literally the language in the title of this paper
2:05:14
there. Still.
2:05:15
Seeing all these Attractive people. It's just that if they're feeling filled
2:05:19
up in air quotes, psychologically, filled up, emotionally filled up,
2:05:24
autonomically, filled,
2:05:26
enhanced in the language that we're using today. By The self-expansion
2:05:30
Narrative will, then the same set of attractive faces appear less
2:05:35
attractive to a given individual.
2:05:38
Now, whether or not this predicts cheating or loyalty, I certainly
2:05:42
can't say that would be very hard to assess in it in neuroimaging.
2:05:45
Aging and they're of course, people
2:05:48
rarely if ever report
2:05:51
accurately. They're cheating Behavior. There's some studies in which confidentiality is assured to the point where people seem to be more trusting and willing to reveal cheating Behavior. But if you look at the statistics on cheating Behavior, it's very hard to track because people lie all the time about their cheating in and outside of the context of psychological and neuroimaging studies, but
2:06:13
I find this study again, the title
2:06:15
Elation, of self-expansion Alters responses to attractive alternative Partners to be absolutely fascinating. Because, again, it points to the fact that the interactions with
2:06:25
our significant others
2:06:27
shapes, our autonomic arousal shapes, our perception
2:06:30
of self
2:06:30
and thereby shapes. Our perception of other potential
2:06:35
Partners in the outside
2:06:36
world, or shuts us down to the potential of other people in the outside world. So when I hear statements such as it's important that you love yourself in order to
2:06:45
Lee fall in love with somebody else or it is when one is not looking for a relationship that they're
2:06:51
most likely to fall in love and form a stable relationship.
2:06:56
I can filter that through these findings to say that it's really the person who needs a lot of self-expansion, stimulating statements or actions coming from other people. That is most prone to seeing
2:07:12
Other potential Partners
2:07:13
out in the world as attractive.
2:07:15
And in this sense, we can return to the autonomic nervous system is kind of a kind of a glass that it can be filled up through various contexts. It can be filled up through our own ability to regulate it.
2:07:27
It can be filled up
2:07:28
through other people's ability to enhance our sense of well-being and in some sense this points to an idea where it is true that the better that we can feel about ourselves in the absence of
2:07:40
any self-expansion.
2:07:43
Input from somebody else
2:07:44
really does place on more stable ground. Such that when we do receive
2:07:48
that, praise, or we do receive those acts of kindness or service or physical touch, or whatever. They are
2:07:53
that we are able to, to further
2:07:56
enhance the way that we feel,
2:07:58
but that we don't necessarily tether all of our feelings of self-worth or self-expansion to that one individual. So you might think that if person a can only receive the self-expansion,
2:08:12
From the statements from the
2:08:13
action of the person they're involved with person be that that will form a very
2:08:18
stable Bond. But what this study points to is the fact that that's a very
2:08:21
unstable bond that person a is actually very susceptible to the attractiveness of others because they're so desperately attached to this notion of self expansion, even if they don't realize it. And
2:08:32
so this really does point to the idea that while it is important to link our autonomic
2:08:38
nervous systems to establish desire,
2:08:40
love and attachment.
2:08:42
That we want to have a stable,
2:08:44
internal representation of ourselves a stable, autonomic nervous system to some degree or another so that we can be
2:08:51
in
2:08:52
stable romantic partnership with another individual. If that's what we're really trying to
2:08:56
do. So, until now, I've been weaving together studies from the field of experimental
2:09:00
psychology and the fields of Neuroscience and particular neuroimaging. But if you recall back to the very beginning of the episode, when I was discussing, how
2:09:12
ow, odors and how hormones and how even birth control can shape people's ratings of attractiveness of others,
2:09:21
you'll realize that there's a deeper layer to
2:09:24
all this, which is that
2:09:26
our biology that resides below our conscious
2:09:30
awareness things like our hormones, things like pheromones, even are shaping the way that
2:09:38
we choose interpret and
2:09:42
Act with other potential romantic Partners or the Romantic partners that we already
2:09:47
have. Now, this cannot be
2:09:50
overemphasized. All right,
2:09:52
no matter how much we would like to create a sort of top-down
2:09:57
description, meaning from the cortex in our
2:10:00
understanding of things
2:10:01
onto what we find attractive, who we find attractive, what we enjoy, what? We don't enjoy in the pursuit and romantic interactions with others.
2:10:10
There, always seems to be an
2:10:12
Indeed, there always is a deeper layer in
2:10:15
which our subconscious processing drives us to find a particular person to be particularly attractive or
2:10:24
in which we have chemistry with
2:10:25
somebody or in which we lack chemistry with
2:10:28
somebody. And I would say that one of the more exciting fascinating and indeed mysterious aspects
2:10:35
of Desire. Love and attachment, are those subconscious processes? Those things that we
2:10:42
Chemistry,
2:10:43
right? I mean, people will report for instance, that somebody smell is just absolutely positively intoxicating for them or that somebody smell is absolutely
2:10:53
repulsive to them. And they don't know why that the
2:10:56
taste of someone's breath. And I don't mean that in any kind of poetic sense. I literally
2:11:01
mean the taste of somebody's breath in. Some cases can be very exciting to somebody and believe or not. We can taste each other's breath. I talked about this in the chemical sensing.
2:11:12
Showed
2:11:13
some months back, but we actually have receptors for taste and smell that engaging coordinated action such that we can't really separate taste and
2:11:22
smell at some level. And this is especially true when it comes to the formation of romantic relationships. And what we call Chemistry now is chemistry.
2:11:31
Absolutely required for forming, stable attachments for love. And for desire. No, of course, they're not but in general,
2:11:41
These are primitive mechanisms that exist in all animals. They exist in special forms in humans, but that they drive us toward behaviors that will
2:11:52
As the theory goes lead to
2:11:54
love and attachment, not always as dr. Fisher pointed out, that
2:11:59
sex. And sex drive is one way to
2:12:00
explore potential, love relationships and to explore potential attachments, which of course, are major Investments that extend. Well beyond, you know, one night or a week or a vacation or even a year when we talk about stable attachments in general that means long-term attachments in humans. Now,
2:12:20
there is a biology
2:12:21
to
2:12:22
Of that chemistry stuff and the studies of oral
2:12:26
contraception and Men, finding women, more attractive at certain phases of their menstrual cycle and women finding men more attractive at certain
2:12:35
phases of the woman's, menstrual
2:12:36
cycle point to the incredible
2:12:39
power of those deeper biological mechanisms.
2:12:43
In the huberman lab podcast, I discussed
2:12:45
both science and science based tools. And so I'd be remiss if I didn't actually cover some of the tools that relate to those deeper biological
2:12:53
mechanisms, how
2:12:56
the hormones testosterone and estrogen are almost always the first
2:13:01
biological chemicals and hormones. That are mentioned in described and explored when thinking about desire and love and attachment to, for that matter since
2:13:12
Attachment stem from desire.
2:13:16
I did an entire episode about the
2:13:19
biology of testosterone. And estrogen, and ways to optimize testosterone and estrogen, you can easily find that episode at huberman, lab.com., It's
2:13:28
time-stamped there. You can find, all sorts of
2:13:30
information about how certain behaviors or absence of behaviors drive up or down testosterone and estrogen. I also dispel some myths about sexual behavior and things like masturbation and
2:13:42
How they relate to testosterone and estrogen, as well as some myths about how those hormones change across the lifespan. I also talked about the role of exercise. I talked about supplementation, and I also talked a little bit about hormone replacement therapy. Although that will be the topic for a future episode. So if you're
2:14:00
interested in the biology of testosterone and estrogen to hormones that absolutely
2:14:04
influence things like libido and desire, please check out that episode as well as what I'm going to talk about in just a moment here. The
2:14:12
Stereotyped version of the hormones testosterone and estrogen are that testosterone drives libido or increases. It AKA sex drive and that estrogen somehow blunted or is not involved in libido and sex drive and that is simply not
2:14:28
the case. As I described in that
2:14:30
testosterone and estrogen optimization episode and is I'll tell you
2:14:33
now. Yes, testosterone and some of its other forms. Like the Hydra testosterone are strongly
2:14:41
related to
2:14:42
IB, do and sex drive in the pursuit and ability to mate. However,
2:14:48
the hormone estrogen
2:14:49
is also strongly associated with libido, and mating behavior
2:14:53
so much. So that for people that either chemically or for some other reason have
2:14:59
very low estrogen libido can severely suffer. So it's a coordinated dance of estrogen and testosterone in both males and females that leads to libido or sex drive. So I absolutely want to
2:15:12
Clear. That it's not a simple relationship between testosterone and sex drive, or estrogen, and sex drive, both are required at appropriate ratios.
2:15:23
Now, with that said, there are things that can shift
2:15:29
libido in both men and women
2:15:31
in the direction of more
2:15:34
desire or more desire to mate, either to seek mates or to mate, with existing
2:15:40
partners, and there's a
2:15:42
A quite solid literature around a
2:15:44
few of those substances. Now, a common
2:15:47
misconception is that because dopamine is
2:15:49
involved in motivation and drive
2:15:52
that simply increasing dopamine
2:15:54
through any number of different mechanisms or tools will increase libido and sex drive. And that's simply not the case
2:16:01
either. It is true that some
2:16:06
level of dopamine or increase in dopamine is required for increases.
2:16:12
In libido, however, because of dopamine's relationship to the autonomic nervous system and because the autonomic nervous system is so intimately involved. No pun
2:16:23
intended in sexual
2:16:25
activity in seeking an actual mating behavior. As I described earlier. It's
2:16:30
actually the case that if people drive their dopamine system too high, they will be in states of arousal that are high enough such that they seek and
2:16:42
want
2:16:43
sexual activity, but they can't actually engage the parasympathetic arm of the autonomic nervous system sufficient to become physically aroused.
2:16:51
Now, there's a whole description of this that awaits
2:16:54
us, in a future episode, but
2:16:56
I'll summarize Now by saying for people that are taking substances just simply to increase dopamine in order to increase libido
2:17:06
that can be a potentially hazardous route to follow because
2:17:11
Depending on whether or not that dopamine level is high enough that it puts them into a mode of seeking
2:17:18
mates or mating,
2:17:20
but they can't adjust their autonomic nervous system during actual mating behavior. What essentially is I'm saying is it can place people into a chronic Pursuit, but an inability to perform
2:17:30
sexually, and this is true for men and women. Okay, so I would just caution people against just thinking, oh, a lack of libido is simply a lack of dopamine. That is
2:17:41
Not the case. It could be from lower levels of dopamine, but it could also be for other reasons.
2:17:47
And so these systems, the signaling systems and these neuro chemicals are very intricate and just simply ramping up, dopamine has actually been
2:17:56
found for instance in and feta mean and cocaine users. There is a phenomenon which they become
2:18:01
hyper aroused but can't perform. Sexually. This is also true for people who take elevated levels of
2:18:07
other recreational drugs or who take antidepressants.
2:18:11
That increase the dopamine system too much, right? Dosage has to be worked out with your physician, with your psychiatrist, such that,
2:18:19
you know, mood is enhanced and the various aspects of healthy well-being, mind and body are enhanced. But not so much. So that that what we call the arousal Arc is locked with the Seesaw in the sympathetic Drive position, such that sexual arousal can't occur.
2:18:36
Okay. So this is a, an important point to make. Because I think that a lot of
2:18:41
People are under the impression that if they
2:18:43
just drive up testosterone increased dopamine and generally get themselves into high states of autonomic arousal that that's going to increase the libido. That's simply not the way, the system works. It's that seesaw in that see-sawing back and
2:18:57
forth. That is the Arc
2:18:58
of arousal that we talked about earlier.
2:19:01
Now, there are substances legal over-the-counter substances that fall under
2:19:07
the category ization of
2:19:08
supplements that do indeed.
2:19:11
Increase libido and arousal.
2:19:13
And so I'm going to talk about some of those in the context of peer-reviewed literature. Now,
2:19:17
I want to be clear. However, that these are by no means
2:19:20
required, many people have healthy libidos or have libidos that are healthy for their
2:19:26
life and what they need and want. And as always in any discussion about supplementation. You absolutely have to check
2:19:33
with your physician. I don't just say that to protect us. I say that to
2:19:36
protect you, your health and well-being is dependent on you doing certain things and not doing others. And
2:19:41
Buddy is different. Nonetheless. There are studies that point to specific substances that are sold over the counter that at least in the United States are legal and that have been shown to be statistically significant in increasing measures of libido. There are many such substances, but three that in particular have good peer reviewed research to support them are Maka
2:20:06
Mac a which is actually a root
2:20:10
tone got
2:20:11
Ali. Also, sometimes called long Jack, I didn't name them forgive me and Tribulus or Tribulus. It's sometimes called I'm going to talk
2:20:19
about each of these in sequence. But
2:20:22
on the whole, the studies on Maka are quite convincing that consumption of 2 to 3 grams per day of maca,
2:20:33
which generally is sold as a powder or a capsule.
2:20:38
Typically consumed early in the day
2:20:40
because it can be
2:20:41
Of a stimulant meaning, it can increase alertness, and you wouldn't want it to interfere with sleep by taking it too late in the day,
2:20:48
but in studies, that include both men and women of durations anywhere from 8 to 12 weeks of athletes and non-athletes and different variations of maca. Turns out there's black, Maka, red, Maka, yellow. Maka, they're a bunch of different forms of Makkah, but that they can increase subjective reports of sexual.
2:21:11
All desire independent of hormone
2:21:14
systems. Meaning it does not seem at least based on the existing literature that
2:21:19
Maca increases
2:21:20
testosterone or changes estrogen at least not on the timescales of these studies were done or with the measures that were performed in these studies,
2:21:27
but that Maka again
2:21:29
consumed in doses of anywhere from 2 to 3 grams per day, has been shown to significantly increase libido.
2:21:37
And in fact,
2:21:39
Those dosages of Makkah have been shown to offset so-called SSS or I induced sexual dysfunction. So there are various routes
2:21:49
to sexual dysfunction.
2:21:51
The ssris are selective serotonin reuptake Inhibitors.
2:21:55
They go by name brands, like Prozac and Zoloft. And there are many others
2:21:58
now on generic forms and so forth. Those don't
2:22:01
always, I should point out lead to sexual dysfunction. There's a
2:22:05
dose-dependent. Some people do quite well on
2:22:08
Ssris and don't have any issues with sexual function. Other people suffer quite a lot from sexual dysfunction, while taking ssris
2:22:17
highly variable. You need to work with
2:22:20
physician a qualified psychiatrist, but nonetheless,
2:22:24
everything I've been saying about Makkah. Thus far has also been
2:22:28
explored in the context of SSRI and do sexual dysfunction. The
2:22:32
paper that I referring to here is a double-blind randomized, pilot dose finding study of maca,
2:22:38
root.
2:22:39
It goes by the name. L. My any these always have fancy names in the Latin names in biology are always cut more complicated, but it's maca root
2:22:47
for the management of SSRI and do sexual dysfunction. First author, is door. Ding dor D ing. This was a study done at Mass general,
2:22:56
which is one of the satellite locations around. Harvard Harvard
2:23:00
Med, is associated with Harvard Med, that found significant improvements in libido when people were taking a pretty low.
2:23:08
It was actually in this case, just 1.5 grams per day up to a
2:23:14
high dose, three grams per day
2:23:15
of Makkah and they were doing this in 20 remitted. Depressed. Outpatients of these are people that had
2:23:21
depression. Their Depression was successfully treated with ssris, but they were suffering from some of
2:23:26
these SSRI related sexual effects and Maka seem to offset some of those effects significantly in this population. The other studies
2:23:35
exploring the lack of effect on serum testosterone.
2:23:38
And in what in adult healthy men was a 12-week. Study, again consuming anywhere from 1 Point
2:23:44
5 to 3 million milligrams, meaning one, excuse
2:23:47
me, 1,500 milligrams to 3000 mg or Placebo. So again, this is 1.5 up to 3 grams of Makkah or Placebo and they raided
2:23:58
sexual desire depression and
2:24:01
other measures such as testosterone in the blood. Again, no change in testosterone or
2:24:07
estrogen estrogen.
2:24:08
All levels in men, treated with Makkah and those treated with
2:24:11
Placebo. But nonetheless, there was a significant
2:24:15
and positive effect on libido with this dosage of 1.5 to 3 grams per day.
2:24:20
Maka? And there are several other
2:24:22
studies that also show this again in people that are taking us or ssris and people that are not taking ssris and in chronically over
2:24:33
trained athletes. This was also found to be the case. So it seems like across the board.
2:24:38
Maca is a fairly useful
2:24:41
supplement for those that are seeking to increase their libido. And there are
2:24:45
fewer studies involving women, but there are a
2:24:48
few such studies that also point to the same general positive effect on libido in women, taking Maca at equivalent doses. To those. I just described, I think it's noteworthy. That Maka supplementation, does not seem to adjust testosterone or estrogen levels to any significant degree, but it does change libido. I think that
2:25:08
To the fact that there are multiple systems in the brain and body that influence, libido, not just testosterone and
2:25:14
estrogen, and indeed. We know that
2:25:15
to be the case, things like Peña, which is a substance found in chocolate. And is a substance that some people supplement is known for instance, to increase sexual
2:25:26
desire, but also the perception of sexual experiences as
2:25:31
more stimulating for instance. So there are a lot of Pathways in the brain, in particular in the hypothalamus, this ancient area of our brain.
2:25:38
That Harbors
2:25:39
neurons and
2:25:40
hormone secreting cells, including neurons that can shape our perceptions of our even just are tactile experience of others and their attractiveness and indeed can shift levels of
2:25:53
Desire. Independent of changing levels of circulating
2:25:57
hormones. Another substance that has been shown to increase libido. Across a range of human. Populations is so
2:26:04
called tone. Got Ali. I've talked a little bit about this before.
2:26:08
Or on the human Lab podcast in reference to testosterone. And I've talked about it extensively as a guest on other podcasts.
2:26:16
Tonga Ali, goes by a number of different names. One of them is exceedingly difficult for me to pronounce its Yuri, coma longifolia. Also called long Jack, but Tonga Ali is the typical name. This is an herb. There's a Malaysian version and an Indonesian version. My understanding is that the Indonesian variety of Tonga. Ali is the
2:26:38
one that is most potent
2:26:40
for its effects on libido.
2:26:43
Previously. I've talked about tone, golly taken in 400 mg per day capsules, as a means to increase the amount of free, meaning, Unbound testosterone. So
2:26:55
testosterone has a both
2:26:57
bound form in an Unbound form. Very briefly. The bound form is bound to albumin in the blood or to so-called sex hormone-binding globulin when it's bound. It can't
2:27:08
The biologically active at many cells. It is
2:27:12
important that some of it be
2:27:13
bound in order to get a sort of time
2:27:15
release and and proper distribution of testosterone through the body. But is the Unbound
2:27:19
free testosterone that can really have its most potent
2:27:22
effects. And there's some evidence that Tonga Ally can increase the amount of Unbound, so-called free testosterone by
2:27:30
lowering sex hormone-binding globulin. Although it is
2:27:34
almost certain that it has other
2:27:35
routes of mechanism as well.
2:27:38
Nonetheless.
2:27:40
There are some reports of Tonga Ali increasing libido. One particular article last author, or I
2:27:48
should say last name
2:27:49
of the first author. Scuse me. This male is Mal. This was published in a in evidence-based complementary and alternative medicine is from 2012. Reports a significant increase in libido and sexual function. There are other such studies and not a lot of them, not as many robust.
2:28:10
Controlled quality, peer reviewed studies, as there are from Makkah, nonetheless, a number of people men and women that I know do
2:28:17
take Tonga Ali and it seems to work well for them. The
2:28:21
question, always comes up around discussion of supplements. Do you need to cycle these things? The only way
2:28:26
to determine that is really to do your blood. Work, monitor, liver enzymes, honor hormone levels, and so forth. So I simply can't say whether or not you need to, or you don't need to cycle them.
2:28:36
Typically Tonga
2:28:38
Ali and Maja are
2:28:39
not
2:28:40
Cycled in any regular kind of way that I'm aware of. But again, you really need to
2:28:45
check with your doctor. If you're
2:28:46
going to initiate taking any of these things
2:28:49
and you certainly should do your best to monitor. Your blood work as well as subjective measures in evaluating whether or not. They're working for you say, for you and so
2:28:56
forth. The third and final substance / supplement
2:28:59
that I want to touch on as it relates to libido
2:29:02
is called Tribulus terrestris. So that's TR. IB u l u s--
2:29:07
interests is TE RRS.
2:29:10
TR is this is a commonly sold over-the-counter supplement for increasing
2:29:17
testosterone for you know
2:29:20
Fitness purposes and so on whether or not it actually does that to a meaningful degree isn't clear, but I'm aware of for peer-reviewed studies that we're focused on both males and females ranging anywhere from 18 years old. All the
2:29:40
We up to
2:29:40
65. Plus they say 65 plus. I guess it could be seven. You could be 80. I don't know,
2:29:45
but a fairly broad age range, where people took anywhere from 750 milligrams per day, divided into three equal doses. So 750 total per day divided into three equal doses of Tribulus or Placebo for 120 days. This particular study was focused on females and according to
2:30:10
The female sexual function index questionnaire, no significant difference between any of the groups, however, free and bioavailable testosterone increased in the group taking Tribulus terrestris. Total testosterone did not reach
2:30:24
statistical significance. So,
2:30:25
there's a sort of the inverse of what we see with Maka where there
2:30:28
do seem to be increases in testosterone,
2:30:31
which would predict that there would be increase in libido. In this case, in this was postmenopausal women. There was no increase in libido. There
2:30:39
was an
2:30:40
In testosterone, I mention it only because there might be instances in which people want to increase their testosterone. It does seem that Tribulus. At least in that population is capable of doing that.
2:30:51
Now, there's a separate study that was done a double blind study lasting anywhere from one to six months. That had a clear and significant increase in libido. Now this was taking six grams. So that sticks. 6,000 milligrams of Tribulus root for
2:31:08
60 days
2:31:09
and it did.
2:31:10
Seemed to increase various aspects of sexual function.
2:31:13
And there was a what appeared to be a substantial 16.3 percent increase in testosterone, but in this particular study because of the variability across individuals that did not
2:31:25
actually arrive at statistical significance.
2:31:27
Now, there were a number of other studies that explored the role of Tribulus in particular in females. And one of those studies was a study that was actually quite short. It was two to four weeks and involved 67 subjects. These were
2:31:43
Objects that had experienced a loss of libido and took Tribulus divided into two equal doses compared that to Placebo and they did see a significant Improvement in
2:31:54
these measures of sexual desire and function on this female sexual function index.
2:32:00
So there is some evidence that Tribulus can be effective in increasing
2:32:03
testosterone in certain
2:32:05
populations in increasing sexual desire and
2:32:08
function in certain populations in particular
2:32:10
in females. I think more studies are certainly
2:32:13
Needed. But these three substances / supplements, Maka, Tonga Ally in particular, Indonesian Tonga, Ali and Tribulus can indeed create significant increases in sexual
2:32:27
desire. And in some cases B, adjusting the testosterone and estrogen system. In some cases, not by adjusting the testosterone and estrogen
2:32:36
system. Again, pointing to the complexity
2:32:39
of neurochemicals and features. That adjust things like libido egg.
2:32:43
A desire, so we covered a lot of material today related to desire love and attachment. And yet I
2:32:49
acknowledge that it is not
2:32:51
exhaustive of the vast landscape. That is the psychology and biology of Desire. Love and attachment nonetheless.
2:32:59
I hope that you found the information interesting
2:33:02
and hopefully actionable in some cases toward the relationships of your past of present and potentially for the relationships of your future,
2:33:12
if you
2:33:13
You're enjoying and or learning from this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube
2:33:17
channel. That's a very straightforward. Zero cost way to support us and it really does help us
2:33:22
in addition. Please subscribe to our podcast on Apple and Spotify. And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us up to a
2:33:28
five star review.
2:33:30
In addition. Please leave us comments, feedback and suggestions for future guests in the comment
2:33:35
section on YouTube. We do read
2:33:38
all of those comments, please. Also check out the
2:33:40
sponsors mentioned at the beginning of today's episode. That is the
2:33:43
Way to support the huberman Lab
2:33:44
podcast. In addition. We have a patreon. It's patreon.com slash Andrew huberman. And there you can support the
2:33:51
podcast at any level that you like,
2:33:54
please. Also follow us on Instagram and Twitter. I teach neuroscience and Neuroscience related tools on both Instagram and Twitter. Some of that material overlaps with material covered on the podcast. Some of it is unique material, only covered on Instagram and Twitter during today's episode and on many previous episodes of The
2:34:10
huberman Lab podcast, we discussed supplements.
2:34:14
Well, supplements aren't necessary or appropriate for everybody. Many people drive
2:34:18
tremendous benefit from them for things like enhancing sleep, enhancing Focus
2:34:22
or as discussed today, for enhancing libido and desire. If you want to see the supplements that I take, you can go to Thorn that's th OU our any.com the letter U / hubermann and there, you can get 20% off any of the supplements that I take and if you navigate deeper into the thorn site through that portal
2:34:41
thorn.com.,
2:34:43
Are you / hubermann, you
2:34:45
can also get 20% off
2:34:46
any of the other supplements that thorn
2:34:48
makes we partnered with Thorn because Thorn has the absolute highest standards with respect to the quality of the ingredients in their
2:34:54
supplements and the Precision of the amounts of those supplements.
2:34:58
Thank you for joining me. For today's discussion about desire love and attachment and last but certainly not least.
2:35:05
Thank you for your interest in science.
ms