PodClips Logo
PodClips Logo
Huberman Lab
Science-Based Mental Training & Visualization for Improved Learning
Science-Based Mental Training & Visualization for Improved Learning

Science-Based Mental Training & Visualization for Improved Learning

Huberman LabGo to Podcast Page

Andrew Huberman
·
35 Clips
·
Apr 24, 2023
Listen to Clips & Top Moments
Episode Summary
Episode Transcript
0:00
Welcome to the huberman live podcast where we discuss science and science based tools for everyday life.
0:09
I'm Andrew huberman. And I'm a professor of
0:11
neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford school of
0:14
medicine. Today we are discussing mental training and
0:16
visualization
0:18
mental training. Visualization is a fascinating process that has been shown over and over again in now hundreds of studies to improve our ability to learn anything. When I say anything, I mean,
0:30
the ability to learn music, the ability to learn and
0:33
perform mathematics,
0:35
the ability to learn and perform motor skills in sport and dance across
0:39
essentially. All domains the
0:41
other incredible thing about mental training of visualization is that I did see, you'll soon. See when you go into the literature that
0:47
is the scientific studies on mental training in visualization, you
0:50
quickly realize that it does not take a lot of mental training visualization, in order to get better at anything. However, that mental training of visualization,
1:00
Has to be performed in a very specific way. And today we will discuss exactly how to do. Mental training visualization in the specific ways that allow it to complement the actual
1:09
performance of a motor or cognitive skill to allow you to learn more
1:13
quickly. And to consolidate that is to keep that information in mind and body, so that you can perform those
1:19
cognitive tasks music, task
1:21
motor, tasks, etc for long periods of time
1:24
without ever forgetting how to do them.
1:26
All of mental training and visualization relies on what I can.
1:29
Siddur really the Holy Grail of our brain and nervous system.
1:32
And that's a neuroplasticity, neuroplasticity is our nervous
1:36
system. Which of course includes the brain, the spinal cord
1:39
and all the connections between the brain and spinal cord, and the
1:41
organs and tissues of the body. And then, all the neural connections, back from the organs and tissues of the body to the brain and spinal cord. So the whole thing in both directions,
1:50
has the ability to change in response to experience
1:53
in ways that are adaptive. That is that allows us
1:56
to do things that we could not do before.
1:58
And by doing those
2:00
Things were by being able to perform those mental operations. We can do better in the world that we live in. We can
2:05
perform new tasks, we can think new thoughts. We can come up with novel solutions, to pre-existing problems that before really
2:13
vexed us and that we couldn't
2:14
overcome. All of that is
2:16
considered neuroplasticity. So
2:18
today, what I'm going to cover is a brief summary of what neuroplasticity is, that
2:22
is how it occurs in the brain and body. This is
2:25
extremely important to understand. If you're going to use Mental training and visualization.
2:29
I'm going to talk about what happens in our brain and
2:32
body. When we do mental visualization, in a dedicated way,
2:35
many people have heard perhaps that when you imagine something happening that your brain doesn't know the difference
2:41
between that
2:42
imagination of the thing happening. And the real thing
2:45
happening, turns out that is not true. It is simply not true. However, there
2:50
is somewhat of an equivalence between a real experience and an imagined experience and we'll talk about the difference between those and how that can be leveraged in order to get the
3:00
Most
3:00
out of mental training and visualization.
3:01
Then I will cover exactly, which types of mental training and visualization work. Best
3:06
across all domains meaning for Music Learning mathematics, solving, puzzles, motor learning Sports Performance, etc, etc. To
3:14
really allow you a template in which you can Plug-In or designate, what you're going to do each day for a brief period of time, in order to accelerate your learning in, whatever you choose. And then I'm going to go into a bit of what happens in the brains of different types.
3:29
It's of people, these different types of people that I'm referring to are people who have more or less of a natural ability to imagine things and
3:37
visualize them. Because it turns out that we vary tremendously from
3:40
one individual to the next, in terms of our ability to mentally visualize and imagine things, and our ability to get better at that over time. And the good news is anyone can get better at mental training and visualization in ways
3:53
that can serve them. Well,
3:54
also briefly touch on the fact that certain people in particular people on the autism spectrum.
4:00
As well as people with synesthesia, as which is the combining of different perceptual experiences. So you may be one of these people are, you may have heard of people that, for instance, when they think of a number. They also just naturally spontaneously think
4:11
of a color and vice
4:12
versa, talk about how that relates to mental imagery and visualization, and the creative process and
4:17
problem solving in general.
4:19
And then finally, what I'll do is I'll recap mental training and visualization from the standpoint of how best to apply mental training of visualization. According to specific
4:27
challenges, things like
4:29
Challenges with public speaking or challenges with sports performance or challenges with
4:34
test-taking Performance challenges, with essentially, anything that will allow you to build specific mental training, visualization practices that our brief that are supported by Neuroscience studies and that are highly effective before we begin. I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and
4:50
research roles at Stanford.
4:51
It is however, part of my desire and
4:53
effort to bring zero cost to Consumer information about
4:55
science and science related tools to the general public in keeping with that theme. I'd like to thank
4:59
The sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is element element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need meaning, sodium, magnesium and potassium, but nothing that you don't meaning, no sugar. And it has the sodium, magnesium, potassium in the ideal, ratios for hydrating and providing
5:15
electrolytes to the cells and tissues of your body.
5:18
So, I use element in my water. When I wake
5:20
up, I like to hydrate right away. So, I'll have an element packet in about 16 to 32 ounces of water. When I wake up, I tend to do the
5:26
same while I
5:27
exercise which I typically do in the morning.
5:29
Times in the afternoon and I'll
5:31
drink another one throughout the day. The great thing about element is
5:33
also tastes terrific. I particularly like the
5:36
watermelon flavor, but frankly, I like all the flavors just
5:39
mixed into again about 16 to 32 ounces of
5:42
water, if you'd like to try element, you can go to drink element, LMN t.com huberman to claim a free element sample pack with your purchase. Again, that's drink element element e.com, hubermann. Today's episode is also brought To Us by Maui Nui venison, which I can confidently say is the most nutrient dense. And
5:59
Red meat available.
6:01
Now, he knew he spent nearly a decade, building, a USDA certified wild harvesting system to help balance deer populations on the island of Maui. The solution they built is extremely powerful because it turns the proliferation of an invasive species into a wide range of
6:15
nutrient-dense products
6:17
from butcher Cuts. So venison,
6:19
steaks and ground venison
6:20
Etc to organ Meats, bone broth, and jerky. I really love all of their
6:25
products. So I love the venison, steaks and
6:27
the ground venison and their bone broth is.
6:29
Is fantastic. It has an unmatched 25 grams of protein,
6:32
per 100 calories, so it's very high density. High quality protein per calorie. So if you'd like to
6:38
try Maui Nui, venison go to Maui Nui venison.com huberman to get 20% off your first order again. That's Maui Nui, venison.com huberman to get 20% off your first order. Today's episode is also brought To Us by eight sleep aids,sleep. Make smart mattress covers with cooling Heating and
6:55
sleep tracking capacity. I've
6:57
talked many times before in this podcast, about the
6:59
fact that
6:59
Deep is the foundation of mental health, physical health, and performance. One of the key features to getting a good night. Sleep is making sure that you get the temperature of the environment you
7:08
sleep in, right?
7:10
That's because in order to fall asleep and stay
7:12
deeply asleep throughout the night. Your core body temperature needs to drop by about 1 to 3 degrees conversely. In order to wake up in the morning, feeling refreshed and
7:20
ready to go, your core body, temperature needs to
7:22
go up by about 1 to 3 degrees. Of
7:25
course, you can adjust the temperature of the room that
7:27
you sleep in. I do hope that people are doing that.
7:29
At,
7:30
but adjusting the temperature of your mattress and your direct
7:32
sleeping environment is also key.
7:34
And with eight sleep, it makes it very easy to
7:36
program the temperature of that mattress and sleep environment
7:39
not just throughout the night. But for specific phases of the night, I started sleeping on an eight Sleep mattress cover well over a year ago, and it's the best sleep that I've ever had. If you'd like to try Aid sleep, you can go to eight, sleep.com huberman and check out the pond Pro cover and save
7:52
$150 at checkout. Eat sleep,
7:54
currently ships in the USA, Canada, UK select countries in the EU and Australia. Again, that's a
7:59
Sleep.com huberman to save $150 at checkout. Let's talk about mental
8:04
training and visualization.
8:07
Now, perhaps surprisingly mental training. Visualization has been
8:10
studied since the late 1800s. It's actually a paper published in
8:14
1880 by gulten called the statistics of mental imagery. So long ago, people were quantifying and trying to understand how is it that people come up with mental images and how they can apply that to learning things more
8:28
quickly and more stably
8:29
Over
8:30
time. Now, as I mentioned earlier, mental training and visualization
8:35
relies on a process that we call
8:36
neuroplasticity, neuroplasticity is a term that many people have heard
8:41
and encompasses many different things. So, broadly speaking, neuroplasticity includes developmental plasticity, which is the sort of plasticity that occurs between about birth and age
8:52
25, and that can be summarized very easily as
8:56
passive plasticity. In other words, the
8:58
sorts of changes that
8:59
Happen in one's nervous system simply by engaging in the world and
9:04
experiencing Life as a child, as a young adult, as an adolescent, and as a twenty-two, twenty-three twenty-four year old Etc. Now, of course, of course. Of course, it is not the case that on your 25th birthday. You close out passive developmental plasticity and start engaging in the other type of neuroplasticity, which is adult neuroplasticity. It's a gradual tapering off of Developmental plasticity that occurs between
9:29
teen age 0 and 25. And for some people might occur somewhere around twenty six for other people around
9:35
23. When we say 25, we're really
9:37
just talking about the average age in which passive plasticity, tapers off. However, starting fairly
9:44
early in adolescence and extending all the way out into
9:48
one's 80s, or 90s or hundreds should one live. That long is the other form of neuroplasticity, which is adult, neuroplasticity, adult, neuroplasticity is very different than developmental plasticity.
10:00
Because it is the sort of plasticity that one
10:02
can direct towards one's own specific desired,
10:05
learning. So if we wanted to get a little bit technical here
10:07
for sake of clarity, not forsake of
10:09
confusion, we would say adult plasticity is really
10:13
about self-directed adaptive, plasticity, and the reason we call it that as opposed to something else, we're simply adult plasticity, is that there are many different forms of neuroplasticity. There is for instance, maladaptive neuroplasticity, that occurs. If one gets a really hard head hit and concussion, there will be changes.
10:29
To the brain and nervous
10:30
system, but those changes the brain and nervous system. Do not allow it to perform better.
10:35
In fact, it often a the brain and nervous systems ability to function and therefore is a
10:38
maladaptive. So I don't want to get overly wordy with a number of different terms here, but I do think it's important to understand that we have developmental
10:45
plasticity again in which the brain and nervous system
10:48
changes simply in response
10:50
to experiencing specific things for better or worse. And there's adult self-directed adaptive plasticity in which one can
10:58
direct specific changes.
10:59
In terms of learning things, cognitively or learning things in terms of motor function. So
11:05
sport, Dance, Etc, or a combination of the two. Now,
11:09
just to really clarify
11:10
what I mean by developmental versus self-directed adaptive plasticity. I mentioned
11:15
that self-directed adaptive plasticity actually can start in
11:18
adolescents, right even though there's ongoing developmental, plasticity. I mean, let's be really direct. The brain of a fourteen-year-old is very different than the brain of that same individual. When that person is 21,
11:29
Because there's ongoing developmental plasticity. However, starting
11:33
at about
11:34
adolescents, we can all
11:36
start to decide what it is that we want to learn and engage in
11:39
self directed adaptive, plasticity.
11:41
Now, the way to engage self-directed adaptive plasticity
11:44
regardless of whether or not you're a thirteen-year-old fourteen-year-old, or you're a
11:47
ninety-year-old or anywhere in between, is that it requires two things. The first thing it requires is focused dedicated attention to the thing that you're trying
11:56
to learn. That's the first step. And that actually,
11:59
triggers a number of different chemical and electrical processes in the brain that are often associated with agitation and frustration, believe it or not, the agitation of frustration is a reflection of the
12:10
release of specific chemicals, in particular norepinephrine and epinephrine also called noradrenaline and adrenaline in the brain and
12:16
body that creates this discomfort and this heightened level of alertness and attention, that many of us don't like intend to back away from, but it is exactly that chemical or I should say neurochemical milieu, which signals to the
12:29
Neurons, the nerve
12:30
cells in the brain and elsewhere in the body that something needs to change. Because if you think about it, if you
12:35
can do something perfectly or if you try and do something and it doesn't cause any
12:39
neurochemical change in your brain and body.
12:42
Well, then there's no reason for your brain and it's connections with the body to change in any particular way, okay? So you need Focus dedicated attention to the thing that you're trying to learn.
12:51
It's often accompanied by agitation frustration
12:54
etcetera. So that's perfectly normal. In fact that's a signal that things are going right,
12:58
meaning they're headed towards learning.
12:59
There's a second component that's really required. For
13:01
self-directed, adaptive plasticity, and that's periods of depressed in particular. A good night, sleep in
13:08
particular, on the night that follows that focused attention to the thing. You're trying to learn. There are now hundreds of studies in both animal models. And in humans showing that it is really during sleep and other states of deep
13:22
relaxation, things
13:24
like meditation and non sleep
13:25
deep breaths. Which I've talked about before on this podcast, but really,
13:28
during our
13:29
A night of sleep that the rewiring of neural connections, that is the actual
13:34
neuroplasticity takes
13:35
place. So the verb neuroplasticity, the rearrangement of connections between neurons really occurs during sleep, in particular, on the first night, following an attempt to learn
13:46
something through this focused attention.
13:48
Now, developmental plasticity, which is passive also requires good sleep, it's slightly different or frankly, it's a lot different in terms of the underlying mechanisms than self-directed adaptive plasticity. But because
13:59
They were mainly talking about how to learn faster
14:01
through mental training and visualization
14:03
and that really Maps more closely on to self-directed Adaptive. Plasticity, just really want to emphasize.
14:08
This two-step process has to be
14:10
focused, dedicated attention, and then there needs
14:13
to be sleep. And in particular, sleep on the first night following that training. Now,
14:19
should you have the unfortunate experience of getting woken up in the middle of the
14:22
night, following trying to learn something or
14:25
should you
14:26
simply not be able to sleep for whatever reason on the night following?
14:29
A bout of learning where an attempt to learn. Do not despair because it turns out that there are what are called second and third night effects. Also, once you sleep,
14:38
you will learn those neuroplastic events. The reordering of connections that we call synapses and the changes that occur in neural circuits,
14:47
that reflects what we call Self direct adaptive, plasticity, that still
14:51
will occur. But ideally, you got a great night sleep on the first
14:54
night following trying to learn and the second night and the third, and so on. And so on.
14:59
Now,
14:59
A few other things that are critical to understand about self-directed
15:02
adaptive, plasticity, that will become
15:04
especially important when thinking about protocols for developing, the ideal mental training and
15:09
visualization process for you.
15:12
And that is that there are different forms of plasticity that occur between neurons. Although the two main forms are what are called long-term potentiation and long-term depression. I just want to queue up right now, that the word depression, is a very loaded word, because the moment people hear the word depression, they think, oh no,
15:29
No, that's bad. But in the case of neuroplasticity, long-term, depression, is simply a change in the connections between neurons. And the excitability between neurons, that in many ways, can be
15:40
excellent. For
15:42
learning things, in particular motor skills, and we'll get into this in more detail in a little bit. But it turns out that a lot of our ability to get better at some sort of motor skill. Involves this thing that we
15:53
call long-term depression, and that's because
15:55
much of what is happening. When we learn a new
15:57
motor skill, is that we are
15:58
depressing or
15:59
Suppressing specific actions in order to generate a very specific coordinated action. Some of the best examples of long-term depression can actually be borrowed from developmental
16:10
plasticity. So, for instance, if
16:13
you've ever sat across from an infant who is trying to eat their meal, so imagine a one and a
16:19
half year old or a two-year-old trying to
16:22
eat some noodles or some soup or any kind of baby suitable
16:26
food with a spoon and
16:29
then,
16:29
They're holding the spoon, or they're trying to hold the spoon. What you'll notice is that their motor movements are terribly uncoordinated, they often will take that spoon to their cheek, or to their eye, to their head. We've all seen these very amusing photos of babies with bowls of food on their head, or with food all over their face or just everywhere to peers that they're basically getting the food everywhere except
16:49
where it's supposed to go, which is in their mouth. And that's because
16:52
their motor movements are not very well coordinated at that age, and they're not very well coordinated. Not because they lack
16:58
sufficient
16:59
Purrs of neural connections, synapses between neurons,
17:02
but rather, because they have too many connections between too many different neurons, the neural circuits that control very dedicated coordinated movement are not there yet. Instead too many neurons are connected to too many other neurons and so they can't generate the
17:18
precise movements that are required in order to get that spoon to their mouth.
17:22
Now, over time they get better at moving the utensil to their mouth.
17:28
Such that hopefully by about age five or six, they are eating, you know, in
17:33
a relatively cleaner way and hopefully by time they're 10 or 11 or 12. They're getting the food into their mouth and not all over their
17:39
face. People learn this to varying degrees. All you have to do is go to a restaurant and watch how people eat and you will see a vast variation in people's coordinated
17:48
movements with utensils. But in general, there's a theme, the younger, the person, the more uncoordinated, their movement of
17:55
utensils, and as they get older, the more coordinated now.
17:58
Course in people that are very old, they have challenges moving objects and their limbs in very smooth
18:04
ways and that has to do with topic that we'll get into when we talk about age-related cognitive decline and motor related dementias, but
18:12
for sake of today's discussion, if you just want to think about what happens with long-term depression, and the development of a motor
18:18
skill, both as a
18:19
baby as an adolescent and as an adult. When you're trying to
18:23
learn a new motor skill,
18:25
is that you are eliminating.
18:28
Movements. And when you are eliminating incorrect movements to arrive at only the correct movements in a
18:34
very reflexive and repeated way. So thank your golf.
18:37
Swing your tennis serve, think serving a volleyball think a child learning to
18:43
crawl and then walk think a child learning to eat with utensils. And the example I gave
18:47
before what's happening in all of those cases is that, yes, certain Connections in the brain are being strengthened or what we call potentiated. They are undergoing long-term potentiation this.
18:58
So-called quote, unquote fire together wire together, Mantra, that was popularized by the great neurobiologist, dr. Carla Schatz. My colleague at Stanford.
19:06
But in addition to that long-term depression, the quieting or the silencing of specific synapses that is connections, between
19:14
neurons is absolutely
19:16
critical for motor
19:17
skill learning. So we have ltp,
19:20
long-term potentiation and LTD long-term. Depression is every bit as important as ltp long-term
19:27
potentiation,
19:28
We're getting better at some sort of motor
19:29
skill and indeed it getting better at some sort of cognitive skill.
19:33
Now, as we hear this, this should be intuitive
19:35
to all of us. If you
19:37
look at somebody's attempt to learn a particular dance step or at somebody's attempt to do a tennis serve. The first time,
19:44
it's all over the
19:45
place now, it's not perhaps all over the place in that they're doing a jumping jack while trying to serve the tennis ball but they're generally arcing the racket to widely on one trial and then there are keying
19:57
it.
19:58
Too close to their body on the next trial. So,
19:59
if we were to draw a line over each one of those trials, we would
20:02
see that there were lines everywhere over time. Whereas once they quote unquote perfect, the tennis serve
20:07
it's going to be line
20:08
drawn directly over line drawing directly
20:11
over line. Meaning the Arc of that tennis serve is going to be very restricted. And that without question,
20:18
has reflected the
20:19
removal or the quieting of
20:21
particular synapses connections between neurons in the brain and body to allow that
20:26
very narrow.
20:28
And directed movement.
20:29
The same is true for learning anything in the cognitive domain. Meaning, if
20:33
you are to learn a language, it is not. Of course, the case that, you know, every word in that language and then you simply remove certain words and arrive at the correct sentence structure that you're trying to achieve.
20:42
But rather you have to suppress your
20:45
native language or if you're a young child, you have to suppress the generation of just kind of random babbling. Sounds turns out babbling isn't random at all but the point is that you have to suppress
20:55
the Annunciation a
20:58
Euler sounds and direct the pronunciation
21:00
of other sounds in order to generate that new language or your ability to speak at all.
21:06
Okay. So we can really think about neuroplasticity as both a building up process in which you increase connection. So called long-term potentiation and a sculpting down or a removal
21:17
of connections process that we're going to call long-term depression. Now
21:21
I have to acknowledge that of
21:22
course there are other forms of neuroplasticity to
21:25
I know they're probably some afficionados listening to this.
21:29
Who will be, perhaps shouting back at whatever device. My voice is coming
21:34
out of. Wait, what about Spike timing-dependent plasticity or what? About paired pulse facilitation? Yes, yes. And yes, there are multiple forms
21:41
of communication between neurons that can strengthen those connections or weaken those connections. But for
21:45
today's discussion, we just broadly want to think about long-term potentiation and long-term depression, because it captures the two most important themes related to mental training and visualization, which is that when we
21:58
form a given cognitive or physical tasks in the real world. So we actually try the dance step or the tennis serve or when we actually try a math problem, where we try and learn some specific knowledge and write it down and remember it that is engaging particular neurons, right? They're firing the releasing chemicals. But it is also actively suppressing the activity
22:18
of other neurons. And we are
22:20
always completely unaware of the ways in which our brain is suppressing certain activity. Okay? So today we have to keep in mind that where there is
22:28
Anything of connections. There is also a weakening of connections and when it comes to mental training and visualization, and here's the really key point with mental training and visualization. You are capturing both processes. Both the potentiation that is the building up and strengthening of connections and the weakening of the connections that are
22:45
inappropriate for the thing you're trying to learn
22:48
and there are different aspects of mental training of visualization. Protocols that really harness the potentiation versus the depression aspect. And today we will
22:58
We'll cover mental training and visualization
22:59
protocols, that
23:00
capture both the potentiation and the depression aspect of neuroplasticity. And in that way, serve as an augment that is a compliment to the actual real-world, cognitive and physical training that you're doing. Because I'll just give this away right now, turns out that mental training and visualization is not a replacement for real-world
23:20
cognitive, or motor Behavior.
23:23
Again mental training, visualization. Cannot replace.
23:27
Real world execution
23:29
of cognitive tasks, or of motor tasks. If you want to
23:32
learn, however, mental training of visualization can and has been shown to be effective for greatly enhancing the speed at which you learn and the stability of that learning
23:41
over time.
23:42
Okay? So let's take a second and really think about
23:45
what's happening in the brain and body.
23:48
When we do mental training or
23:49
visualization, in fact, we can do a little experiment right now, that is not unlike many of the classic experiments looking at what's happening in the brain.
23:57
Embody treat mental training visualization in which I just
24:01
ask you to close your eyes and imagine a yellow
24:05
Cube.
24:07
And next to that yellow cube is a red rose.
24:10
And
24:11
perhaps I also asked you to float or fly up above the cube and the rose, and look at them from the
24:18
top, top down.
24:20
And then I tell you to fly back around and land behind those and look at them from the perspective
24:25
of behind that yellow Cube. And
24:27
That red
24:28
rose. Okay, now what the data tell us is that most people will be able to do that. Most of you will be able to do that
24:35
to some degree or another regardless of your attention span whether or not you have ADHD or not. Most of you will be able to do that to some degree or another. We also know
24:44
from neuroimaging studies in
24:45
which people are placed into a functional magnetic, resonance imaging
24:49
scanner that during the sort of visualization you just did or that described that your visual cortex and Associate areas
24:57
Quote, light up, they become very active in similar but not identical ways to how they would
25:03
light up and be activated, were you to actually look at a yellow cube and a red rose
25:07
on a screen and perhaps Fly Above them,
25:10
virtually of course, and land behind them virtually. Of course, or if you were to actually
25:15
look at a yellow Cube and red rose in the real world, right in front of you, on a table, then you know, get up on your tippy toes and look down on them from the top and then walk around
25:23
the table and look at them from the other side.
25:24
So, there is some degree of what we
25:27
Call perceptual, equivalence between real-world experiences, digital experiences and imagined meeting with
25:33
our eyes closed, just in Our Minds Eye
25:35
experiences. This is true, not just of vision, and what we call the visual
25:40
domain, but also the auditory domain. Okay? So, for instance, I could play for you, a short motif of a song. Let's just pick something that I think most people know goodness, I'm a terrible musician and even worse
25:50
singer but let's just take the opening to
25:52
AC/DC's Back in Black, right? I think I can do that when it's
25:55
like, duh duh.
25:58
Okay, got it. That's the actual sound, although admittedly a dreadful version of the great AC/DC song Back in
26:05
Black. But now I ask you to close your eyes
26:08
or we could keep them open and just
26:09
imagine that don't done. Okay. Or for instance, I place you in a quiet room so you close your eyes and ask you to imagine the opening to AC/DC's black and black, but ask you to pause it halfway through what you would find.
26:27
It again, is that most people somewhere between 90 and 95 percent of people would be able to do all the sorts of things that described right Cuban, Rose a CD Back in Black, even a somatosensory task. I imagine you to imagine what it's like to
26:42
touch felt or to touch chinchilla hair or something like that. A chinchillas hair ideally alive, chinchilla sitting, still those little critters move really, really fast, but they have very very soft hair Hai hair density, So
26:53
Soft. Okay, most people can do that.
26:58
About 5 to 15 percent of people are less able to do that. And there's a small percentage of people in that 5 to 15% that simply cannot do it at all but just cannot visualize. Well, we'll talk later about these people. They have what's called a Fantasia and inability to mentally visualize, but most people are actually pretty good at visualizing things when they are told what to visualize
27:22
and and this is a really key
27:24
point. And if what they're told to visualize is,
27:27
Very simple and the whole visualization
27:30
is quite brief lasting on the order of about 15 seconds to generate the visualization in the auditory, or in the visual aspect of one's mind, eye or ear, if you will, and if it's repeated over and over
27:44
what's far harder for everybody to do. And in fact, what most people simply cannot do is Imagine long extended scenes and stories in their mind that go on for minutes and minutes and involve a lot of different.
27:57
Stimuli this is a really key point. In fact, as we start to home in on ideal mental training and visualization
28:04
protocols, I'd like to establish this as the first principle of mental training of visualization
28:09
which is that, if you are going to use Mental training and visualization to its best effect
28:15
in order to engage neuroplasticity and learning,
28:17
you need to keep those visualizations. Quite brief really on the order of about 15 to 20 seconds or so and pretty darn sparse.
28:27
Meaning not including a lot of elaborate
28:31
visualization not including a lot of sequences of motor steps.
28:35
What I mean our motor sequences,
28:37
if you're trying to learn something in terms of physical
28:39
movement or visual sequences, or auditory sequences, if you're trying to learn things terms of music or Dance Etc that can be completed and repeated in 15
28:51
seconds or less.
28:53
Now later I'll give you a couple of specific examples but if you want to use
28:57
Will training and visualization understand. This is the
29:00
key first principle. They have to be
29:02
very short visualizations that you can repeat over and over and over
29:05
again with a high degree of accuracy.
29:07
So you don't want to embark on a mental training of visualization Paradigm in which it
29:12
involves a lot of elaborate stimuli. And you have to think really hard and work really hard. Even if you're in that category
29:18
of people who can do mental visualization,
29:21
pretty naturally, and easily. Now if
29:23
you're somebody who can't do mental visualization, in fact, if you're somebody who has full-blown
29:27
A Fantasia or the inability to mentally visualize. Well, then it's, especially important that you make those mental trainings and
29:33
visualizations, really brief, and very, very simple. I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge one of our sponsors. Athletic greens, athletic greens.
29:42
Now called a G1 is a vitamin mineral
29:45
probiotic drink that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs. I've been taking athletic greens since 2012 so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking athletic Greens in the reason I still take athletic greens
29:57
Once or usually twice a day, is that it gets to be in the probiotics that I need for gut health. Our gut is very important, it's populated by gut microbiota that communicate with the brain, the immune system, and basically all the biological systems of our body to strongly impact, our immediate and long-term health, and those probiotics and athletic greens are optimal and vital for microbiotic health.
30:20
In addition, athletic greens contains a number of adaptogens vitamins. And minerals that make sure that all of
30:25
my foundational nutritional needs are
30:26
met and
30:27
And it tastes great. If
30:29
you'd like to try out, let it greens. You can go to athletic greens.com huberman and they'll give you five free travel packs that make it really easy to mix up athletic greens. While you're on the road, in the car, on the plane etcetera and they'll give you a year's supply of vitamin D3 K to again, that's athletic greens.com huberman to get the five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 k 2.
30:51
Now in order to develop the best mental training in
30:53
visualization protocols for
30:54
you. Let's go a little bit deeper into what the research says.
30:58
About mental visualization. Now the classic work on mental visualization, really hinges on a number of different researchers and their work. But in particular, Roger Shepard, who did this work at Stanford, and Steven kosslyn who's now
31:11
at Harvard, of course,
31:12
others in the field. But it's really the work of Shepherd and kosslyn, the lay the foundation for our
31:17
understanding of what happens in the brain when we mentally visualize something.
31:23
Shepherd do these incredible experiments in which he had students? Mentally visualize simple
31:29
objects like a square like a triangle
31:31
and he measured how long it
31:33
took them to do that. Now, of course
31:35
at the time when he did these experiments,
31:37
there were no sophisticated brain Imaging devices. And machines like fmri,
31:43
however everything I'm about to describe has been later confirmed using
31:47
things like fmri.
31:49
What Shepherd did? And what he found is that if
31:53
People were told to visualize, very simple objects, they did it pretty quickly.
31:58
However, if they were told to visualize more complex objects
32:02
or importantly to rotate those objects in their minds, I will then it took
32:07
longer for them to perform those mental
32:09
visualizations. Now, many of you might
32:11
think, duh, if I have to just imagine a triangle or a cube that's going to be very easy and very fast, whereas if I have to rotate that triangular cube, in my mind's eye, that's going to take more time and indeed that is somewhat of a duh.
32:23
Except and this is so very important except that what Shepherd and his
32:28
colleagues found? Is
32:29
that how long it takes somebody to generate and rotate a given visual image scales directly with the complexity of
32:38
that image. In fact,
32:40
causal and did some experiments I think illustrate this even better and here's the
32:43
experiment. I love this experiment. I think you'll love it too because
32:47
it illustrates something. So,
32:48
fundamentally important about how our brains work not just for sake of mental training in visualization.
32:53
Which is our brains work at all.
32:57
He showed people a picture of a
32:58
map, so a map drawn on a piece of paper. This was a map of an island, it included things like a loading dock for some boats. It had a location for getting
33:08
food on the island, had some trees, add some other small landmarks drawn out. And people looked at
33:14
this and memorized it
33:16
or in other experiments. They just had people
33:18
imagine this island and the location of these different landmarks on the island. So it didn't really matter which
33:24
but then he had people
33:26
And moving, or walking from one location on
33:30
the island to another. So they say, okay, you're at the loading dock
33:33
now, move to
33:35
the restaurant, okay, you're at the
33:36
restaurant. Now move to the palm tree here, on the North Shore of the island. Now, go around the side of the island clockwise to arrive
33:44
at the bay on the southwest corner, this sort of thing,
33:49
but Colin found was absolutely
33:51
incredible. What he
33:52
found was that the amount of time that it takes people to
33:56
move from one location on the map to another scaled linearly directly with the actual physical location between those objects on the map. So for those of you that can understand or into the importance of
34:11
what Shepherd and causal and showed great.
34:14
I'm guessing however that for most people out there, you're still grasping it like, okay, interesting. You know how things happen in the real world, dictates how they happen in Our Minds Eye. But I want to make sure that I really nailed.
34:26
Go home the importance of this for everybody. The importance
34:29
of this is that when we look at something in the
34:32
real world, so if I look at the pain in front of me, I'm holding up my pen. For those of you that are listening, just holding up my pen in front of me, I move it to the right and back and
34:39
forth. What's happening is I'm activating, or I'm triggering the electrical activity of neurons, which we can think of kind of as pixels
34:47
in my eye. Okay. So it's leftward or rightward motion for me and back and
34:52
forth and those are getting activated in there. Sending signals up to my
34:55
visual cortex.
34:56
And that information is processed at a given speed.
35:01
What the visualization experiments that
35:03
Shepherd and causal and others did
35:05
show is that the processing speed of imagined
35:10
experiences is exactly the same as the processing speed of real experiences
35:16
and the spatial relationship between imagined. And real experiences
35:20
is exactly the same as well.
35:22
Put simply when we imagine
35:24
something in our minds eye or mind's ear,
35:27
we are Imagining the real thing happening. And when I say the real,
35:31
A thing, it's not the
35:32
obvious real thing. Of course, if you're imagining something, that's the thing, you're
35:35
imagining. What I mean is that your brain at the level of
35:38
neurons is behaving exactly the same way
35:42
and this needn't have been the case okay there could have been a result. For instance, that if people were asked to visualize a cube and rotate it from, you know, flip it from top to bottom, okay, so put the top that's upward on the table, now down on the table and so forth or to migrate around the eye.
36:01
And you know counterclockwise going from you know the northern coast
36:04
all the way down to the southern coast clockwise and then back up to the northern coast
36:09
that they could have just done it really quickly like all in one second. But that's not what happens. They always match the speed at which they do things in
36:18
their minds eye
36:19
to the same speed that they do them in the real world. So, in telling you this, what I'm saying, is that mental visualization at the neural level is identical to real-world events.
36:31
So when you've heard that, when we imagine something, it's identical in
36:34
terms of our brains experience of it and our bodies experience of it as when we actually experience something. That is true at the neural level.
36:43
However, when it comes to learning and improving performance in the cognitive or physical domain, they are not equivalent. So, this is the second principle of mental
36:52
training and visualization. As you recall, the first
36:55
principle of mental training and visualization was that in order to make it effective, it needs to be very brief and
37:00
very simple.
37:01
And repeated over and over again.
37:03
The second principle of mental training and visualization is that while yes, mental training and visualization recaptures, the same patterns of neural firing in the exact same ways as real-world behavior and thinking, it is not as effective as real-world behavior. And thinking, in other words, if you want to learn something, the ideal situation is to combine real training
37:28
in the physical world, with mental training.
37:31
Me
37:31
and I'll talk about exactly how to do that and
37:33
in what ratios a little bit
37:35
later. Now there's a really incredible set of experiments that illustrate why it is that mental training and visualization can be extremely effective. But that it's always going to be most effective when combined with
37:47
real-world training and experiences.
37:49
The experiments that I'm talking about involved, the use of what are called bi-stable images
37:53
or impossible figures.
37:56
Some of you are probably familiar with impossible figures. These are figures or objects that when you look at them.
38:01
These odd features like you're not sure where they
38:04
stop and where they start, and where they
38:05
end one. Good example, would be the
38:08
so called moebius strip. The
38:10
Mobius strip is literally a strip or a line that is contiguous. It goes up, and it loops around and then it curves around and then it goes back and it just continues and continues. And when you look at it, you can never really tell where it starts and where it stops because it doesn't have any of the features that allow you to see what's the front and
38:28
what's the back in any kind of stable way.
38:31
Another example of an
38:32
impossible figure would be, you know, a little set of Cubes that look like they're coming out toward you maybe with a little Bend in them, going up at a right angle perhaps, but then, if you look at it a little bit longer, that little piece that's facing up, looks like it's in front and you can't really tell what's in front and
38:48
what's in back. And
38:49
so it's called an impossible figure because you don't really know how to frame it in your
38:54
mind to tell what's closer to you and what's further
38:56
apart by stable, images are somewhat similar, although different
39:01
In the sense that they
39:03
typically are simple Silhouettes. So, for instance, the faces vases bi-stable image is perhaps the most famous of these. Where you look at this image. It's very simple, and it looks like two vases. But then you look at it a little bit longer. And you realize that, you're looking at the side angle, or the profile of two faces, looking at one another. And when you see those two faces looking at one another, you can't see the vases at the same time, but then if you decide to see the vases again, you can see the vase is again, but the faces disappear. So it's bi-stable me.
39:31
Being that you
39:31
can't see the faces and bases at the same time
39:34
and impossible figures and bi-stable images, are capturing the fact that your visual cortex and some of the associated areas that
39:43
compute visual scenes in your world
39:46
are essentially trying to recreate whatever it
39:48
is that's out in front of them and that's effectively what your visual system does. It's very good at recreating visual images in your brain in your mind's eye. As if you think about it. Even with your eyes open, your brain is just creating an abstract representation of
40:01
What it thinks is out there.
40:03
But that when it comes to assigning, an
40:05
identity to something like, oh, that's a face, or oh, that's a
40:08
vase. That is constrained by different, neural circuits by different areas of the brain and somehow those circuits can't be coactive. We cannot see the faces and the vases at
40:20
exactly the same time. We can switch back and forth really quickly
40:23
just as we can switch back and forth really quickly. When we're looking at the impossible figure and think, okay, that's the front of it. That's the back. No wait. That's the back.
40:30
That's the front.
40:31
Intense going back and forth but we can't see them both at the same time. No one can see them both at the same time, okay, we know this from brain Imaging. Studies
40:39
now impossible figures in bi-stable images. Can be seen, right? You could look them up right now
40:44
on your phone or computer or I could show you pictures of them on paper, right in front of you,
40:49
and you can do these sorts of perceptual experiments of
40:52
telling people. Look at the face, look at the vase. Look at the front of the cube, now make it at the back of the
40:56
cube, and they can do this somewhat deliberately.
40:59
However, and this is, I think so very interesting to understanding how mental training
41:04
and visualization does, and does not support real-world
41:07
learning. If you try to
41:10
imagine a bi-stable image,
41:13
you can't do it. In fact, no one can do it until they do something else, okay? So for those of you are saying, wait, I can do it, I can do faces, vases in my mind's eye. I promise you that the neuroimaging disputes your
41:27
belief, okay? And
41:28
Ports. The idea that we can see real world by stable images, we can see real
41:33
world impossible figures,
41:34
but when we try and imagine those in Our
41:36
Minds Eye, we simply can't do it. We
41:38
can't do the perceptual
41:39
shift in Our Minds Eye, we can't switch back and forth, between phases and vases. However,
41:46
and I just have to chuckle because I think these experiments are so clever. If I have you trace or Draw
41:53
With a pen on a piece of paper and impossible figure or the faces, vases bi-stable image. And then I ask you
42:03
to imagine that bi-stable image or impossible figure and to switch back and forth, you are able to do it.
42:10
So what that illustrates is that it's the combination of imagined and real-world experiences, real motor movements real perceptual experiences combined with motor movements combined with what you imagine in your mind's eye.
42:23
That really gives you the most depth and flexibility
42:27
over your mental visualization. And in doing so we can really stamp
42:32
down a third principle of mental training and visualization, which is that your mental training and visualization will be far more effective. If you are performing the exact same or very similar mental,
42:45
and physical tasks in the real world,
42:48
okay? So, first principle is mental training. Visualization needs to be simple and brief and
42:53
That s is that mental training and visualization is not a replacement for real-world
43:00
motor training or cognitive training. It's an augment, it's an addition that can really help and the
43:05
third principle of mental training and visualization is that you need to combine mental training and visualization with real-world behaviors and experiences that are very,
43:16
very similar. It was a
43:18
brief, but I think really relevant to side. One of the things that also makes mental training and
43:23
Visualization. More effective is when we assign cognitive labels to what's going on when we visualize. So, what I mean is that people are much better at manipulating faces and vases in their minds
43:34
eye, of course only once they've drawn them out physically with their hand, as I mentioned before,
43:39
then they are manipulating abstract objects
43:43
like impossible figures
43:45
in part because by labeling them faces and vases.
43:50
People are able to capture a lot of other neural Machinery, that's related to faces and basis.
43:54
In fact, we have entire brain areas on both sides of the brain devoted to the processing of faces of the fusiform face area. We have other areas in our brain that are involved in processing of 3D
44:04
objects, but faces are of particular value. There's a, there's a value to understanding what a face is as opposed to a non face. And there's a value to understanding what a particular face is. In fact, the simplest way to put this is that the human brain is in many ways a face recognition
44:20
In an expression of
44:21
faces recognition, machine, of course, does other things, but it is exceptionally good at that. Unless
44:26
you're in a profession
44:28
in which the relationships between 3D objects in your ability to manipulate them as exceedingly
44:32
important, you're not going
44:34
to have a lot of neural real estate specifically devoted to that. Some people will be better at it some people worse
44:40
but when it comes to faces, unless you have a condition, like prophetic, no Co which is an inability to recognize say, famous faces
44:48
and distinguish them from non famous faces.
44:50
Or if you have some sort of face recognition deficit, which about anywhere from one, perhaps two, three percent of
44:56
people out there have they're just terrible at recognizing faces and by the way,
45:00
there's about half a percent of people out there that
45:02
are what are called super recognizers that can recognize faces in a
45:06
large crowd. They
45:07
can recognize specific faces. Even from just partial profiles. By the way, these people are extremely valuable to Securities agencies and security agencies are very good at finding these people
45:18
machines are quickly.
45:20
Getting better or at least as good as super
45:23
recognizers. But the best super recognizers are still better than the best AI and machine algorithms out there.
45:29
But the point is that in your mind's eye, you are better able to manipulate specific objects or to see things more clearly and with more specificity when it has a label that you recognize
45:44
from your real-world experience as opposed to abstract or
45:47
fictional labels. Okay? Against
45:49
Ring home, the idea that what you experience in the real world, really serves to support
45:55
your mental imagery and
45:56
therefore, the key importance of experiencing and doing things in the real world
46:01
and supporting that with mental training and visualization. And not just relying on mental training and visualization
46:06
and the tangent here that's a little bit of fun. And I don't think we've ever talked about before on this podcast. Is that of UFOs? Unidentified flying objects. There's a lot of people out there who think that they've seen UFOs. I guess. Technically they have because a
46:20
Was identified flying object. And if it's unidentified at least to them than it is indeed a UFOs. I guess the question is whether or not, we're the dispute rather is whether or not, those UFOs are actually flown by aliens or control by aliens, think that's where the dispute laws.
46:33
But you can imagine how if somebody sees
46:35
an object in their environment and decides? Ah,
46:38
that's a UFO. Okay? Remember these faces vases or these impossible figures if they say, oh, that thing is a UFO as opposed to something else they see. In other words, the face, not the vase. Well, that's stamped.
46:49
Sit down as a memory in their visual system and related systems. And then in their minds eye, they are seeing the UFO. They're not seeing the
46:59
other thing that it could possibly be okay. So it's stamped on a very specific memory.
47:04
So the point here is that mental training and visualization, relies on, not just the physical Contours and the exact spatial profiles in the
47:13
speed of movement of particular things that we experience in the real world,
47:16
it also heavily depends on the cognitive labels and the
47:19
Asians we make about the things that we see
47:22
and this will become very important as we build up toward our, fourth
47:26
principle of mental training visualization, which is
47:28
that our cognitive labels, that is what we decide is
47:32
happening. When we do mental training, visualization, turns out to be very
47:35
important. Now, this is not simply to say that you can decide, okay? I want to learn how
47:40
to play piano and so I'm going to tell
47:43
myself that a particular chord. I imagined in my mind's eye is identical to the real world chord just because I decided is
47:49
the brain doesn't work that way. It's not
47:51
possible to just lie to yourself and learn better as a consequence of the lies. You tell yourself, however,
47:57
what this tells us is that it is very, very important that your mental training and visualization accurately recapitulate, the real
48:06
world training that you're doing.
48:07
So we are going to stamp down a fourth principle of effective mental
48:11
training and visualization. Based on what we know from the scientific literature
48:15
is that your mental training and visualization should assign
48:19
Labels to what you're doing. That can be matched to real-world training and experiences. Now these can be somewhat abstract. So for instance, if you're trying to learn a particular
48:32
aspect of the golf swing, okay? So let's say that you're working on your golf swing seems to be there. A lot of people out there working on their golf
48:38
swing and you're going to do some mental training in visualization in order to improve your golf swing. We already know again, let's just March through them. That your mental training, visualization needs to be brief and simple. It needs
48:49
Has to be
48:50
the same. Or in fact, it will be. We can say the same as your real world golf
48:54
swing. In other words, it will take you exactly the same amount of time to perform that golf swing in your mind's eye as it would in the
49:00
real world. Incredible. Right. Again, something that maybe is take a little bit of time to sink in, but once it does, it'll be like, wow, the brain is really an incredible machine
49:08
and that third principle that you still have to do golf
49:12
swings in the real world. In addition to the mental training of golf swings and
49:16
forth that if you want that mental training, visualization,
49:20
To really improve your golf swing. You're going to have to name or apply an identity to the specific golf
49:27
swing or aspect of the golf swing that you're practicing. So this could be abstract, you could call it mental
49:32
training and visualization of golf swing 1A
49:35
and you can imagine your mind's eye, you know, the perfect golf swing over and over and over and over.
49:40
But then when you're in the real world you're also going to have to call that.
49:44
Either out loud or just to yourself, golf
49:46
swing one, a okay, as opposed to a
49:49
putt, which might
49:50
B, 1
49:50
B, so naming, and giving it an identity to a real-world skill and applying the same name or identity to the mental version of that the visualization of that can enhance the mental
50:05
training of visualization in significant ways.
50:07
So when we apply identities or names to these mental
50:10
trainings and visualizations and again,
50:13
provide that they are brief and repeated and so on, WE greatly enhance the amount of neural Machinery in the brain and body.
50:20
That we are able to
50:20
recruit. When we go to perform those real-world
50:25
golf swings, and golf pots in here, just replace golf swing and golf. But with anything that you're trying to learn, you're able to recruit a lot more neural machinery and greatly increase the probability of proper
50:36
execution. So before we go any further, I want to share with you a couple of incredible
50:40
aspects of mental visualization, that
50:43
really can be harnessed and applied toward mental
50:46
training and visualization. Some
50:48
of these were done by Roger Shepard.
50:50
Ed and his graduate students and postdocs some work done
50:52
by Steve kosslyn and by
50:54
others. What these experiments really show is that mental training and visualization is capturing many. Many of the exact same features of
51:02
real-world behavior and perceptions, not all of them but many of them. So
51:06
for instance, if I tell you to close your eyes
51:10
and imagine
51:13
A ceiling that has tiles that are black and white checkered
51:17
tiles. You know, one black tile one white tiled. For
51:20
instance, we know, based on experiments where we measure I've movements Behind Closed eyelids that people tend to move their
51:26
eyes up when they are imagining things above them such as the ceiling. Whereas, if I tell you to imagine things down on the floor, like
51:32
you're taking a hike. And you're looking
51:34
for rattlesnakes, actually, just recently, I experienced because it's spring here in, California. Rattlesnake along a hiking
51:40
trail, it's really quite beautiful. Although I
51:43
have
51:43
Confess I enjoy keeping my distance. Don't like snakes very much, I don't dislike snakes but I prefer not to interact with them. Unless I have to
51:52
if I have you imagine that rattlesnake depending on your
51:54
relationship or thoughts about rattlesnakes number of things will happen in your brain. Of course, activation the limbic system or not for instance,
52:02
but what I know is that regardless of how you feel about snakes,
52:06
Most of you will move your eyes
52:08
down when imagining a snake.
52:11
Hey, it might be subtle, it might be fast, but statistically, that result
52:16
shows up as opposed to, when I imagine, where I ask you to imagine something above, you tend to move your eyes up,
52:22
in addition to that, if I tell you, for instance, to imagine an elephant and a mouse next to one another, you presumably have some real world,
52:29
understanding about the relative sizes of elephants versus mice. Elephants generally are bigger than mice. Thank goodness.
52:37
Mice are smaller than elephants. If I asked you to tell me about the details of that Mouse is face. So for instance, can you see its whiskers the processing time required for you to do? That is much longer than the processing time required. If I say, tell me what the position of that elephant's trunk is
52:57
now, why would that be so? Okay, the
53:00
position of the elephant's trunk, wasn't something that I told you. It wasn't dictated by me, it's in your mind's eye. Maybe you don't even know. And you have to
53:06
Searching for it.
53:08
But what we do know is that if I tell you to look at a small object
53:12
in your mind's eye versus a larger object. So for instance, the mouse versus the elephant, it takes
53:16
longer for you to do that. In other words, just as with the map experiment, the distance between things on a map is conserved in your mind's eye as a linear relationship. Takes longer to go far distances between things on a map in your mind, then it does to go shorter distances.
53:36
It's also the case that it takes you longer to look at the details of a small object versus a large object. Because why, because you art zooming, in, in your mind's eye, again, all of which speaks to the equivalence of mental imagery with real-world imagery and perception. And as I mentioned earlier, and as we'll see in a moment, this also extends into the motor domain, it takes you longer to perform Complex Motor sequences in your mind's eye. Then it does simple motor sequences just
54:06
It would in the real world
54:08
and if you're saying of course of course of course. Well then great. Then we've really underscored the point which is that when you imagine things, it is not exactly the same but it is very very much the same as
54:20
actually doing or perceiving those things in the real world
54:24
and the fifth principle of effective mental training and visualization is this notion of equivalence of
54:29
mental imagery versus Real World perception and behavior. These are the experiments as you recall. Where
54:36
If people are told to look for clouds in their mental visualization, they tend to look up or if they're looking for something on the floor, they tend to look down. Even Behind Closed eyelids. Now, this can be applied toward
54:48
building, an especially effective mental training. In visualization protocol. If you deliberately move your eyes in the direction of the thing or things that
54:57
you are trying to recapitulate in your mind in your visualization, that is,
55:01
you don't necessarily have to include this step, but mental training visualization is going to be more effective.
55:06
Active. If you do because with consciously generated eye movements again even Behind Closed eyelids, you are bringing about more of the neural circuitry that one
55:17
would experience if you were to perform that particular cognitive tasks or motor tasks in the real world which as I mentioned before, in principle number three, you need to be doing anyway. Separately from your mental training and visualization. So what we're talking about here is thus far
55:30
five principles of mental training, visualization that are well established from the scientific research literature.
55:36
In fact, I haven't mentioned this quite yet and I'll refer to some other references. But there's a wonderful systematic review of a large. Number of studies that have looked at mental training and visualization, what's effective what's less effective across a bunch of different disciplines that include education medicine, music, psychology and sports. We will provide a link to this paper in the show. No captions, but the title of the paper is best practice for motor imagery. A systematic literature review on motor imagery
56:02
training elements in five different disciplines as the title suggests. It's mainly for
56:07
Motor imagery training,
56:08
but it extends into
56:10
music, which of course, involves motor training and execution.
56:14
But as well as education, this review establishes, a number of different important things. I'm going to read off some of the key or highlight takeaways. For instance I
56:23
described principle one of effective mental training and visualization which is that the visualization be brief and it be simple and it be repeated. May I ask how many times that very brief 5 to 15. Second
56:36
exercise.
56:36
Size of going through. Some routine
56:39
should be repeated, well, different Studies have used different ranges of let's call them repetitions in a given
56:45
training session. But the number that seems to be most effective is somewhere between
56:50
50 and 75 repeats per session. That brings about the question of how long one should rest between each repeat. This
56:59
gets a little tricky depending on what you're trying to do. Remember that
57:03
we have this threshold of about 15 seconds for completion.
57:06
Entire
57:07
motor sequence, let's say what you're trying to do. Like a golf swing takes you. Five
57:11
seconds to imagine in your mind's eye from the point where you, let's just say, have the ball on the tee, bring the the golf club up, you might reposition your feet, just a little bit, you know, that kind of little wiggle that golf golfers do, and then the swing,
57:24
if that whole thing, takes five seconds in your mind's eye and
57:28
roughly five seconds in the real world. Well, then you'd be able to repeat it, of course, three times in 15 seconds, that would be
57:34
one repetition.
57:36
Even though you're doing it three
57:37
times so there's one 15-second Epic as it's sometimes called EPO CH epoque. And then you would rest for an approximately equivalent amount of time, 15 seconds or so. And then repeat and rest 15 seconds or so. And then repeat, rest 15 seconds and then repeat again,
57:57
three golf swings within that 15
57:58
seconds. Rest, 15 seconds, three golf swings of them, that 15 seconds, rest. 15 seconds. Truth told these pox and these raspberries do
58:06
Need to be exact. You
58:08
could imagine, for
58:09
instance, that you get
58:12
three repetitions of the Swing within 14 seconds. Well, then, do you do another one or do you wait until the end of that 15
58:17
seconds? I encourage you not to obsess too much about those sorts of points. Rather
58:22
you want to do as many repeats as you can in about a
58:26
15-second
58:27
epoque, and then rest for
58:29
about 15 seconds and then repeat for a
58:31
total of fifty to
58:33
seventy five repetitions, which
58:35
might not sound like a lot.
58:36
To some of you might sound like an awful
58:38
lot to others of you to me, it sounds like a lot, you know,
58:42
50 repetitions of something. And where you're trying to concentrate in your
58:44
mind's eye on getting something
58:46
accomplished over and over over
58:48
again, in exactly the same way. Might seem like a
58:50
lot. We know, based on the learning literature that your ability to successfully, perform something in the real world, will
58:58
lend itself to better performance of that thing in the imagined world within your mind's eye. That's also one of these sort of does
59:05
but if
59:06
You're trying to get better at something that you've never performed before. You really should know that the mental training visualization is probably not the best
59:14
augment to that real-world training until you're able to perform it successfully in the real world. At
59:20
least some of the time mental training visualization can be effective however
59:26
at increasing the accuracy or the frequency at which you can do that real-world behavior. So if normally you're only getting the correct swing or you're only hitting the golf ball,
59:36
To correctly, say 10% of the time, mental training and visualization can really help bring that number up. But it is important that you are able to successfully complete that motor tasks in the real
59:46
world, similarly, for performance of cognitive tasks. So,
59:50
for instance, I'm speaking a new language, you might ask. Well, gosh, what, what in the landscape of speaking? A new language can be restricted to 5 to 15 seconds where I could repeat it anywhere from one to three times in a given epic and then rest and then keep repeating 50 to 75 times.
1:00:07
Well, there I would encourage you to pick something that you are able to do, perhaps very
1:00:11
slowly. So to
1:00:12
speak a particular sentence, but with
1:00:15
some challenging, getting the accent in the
1:00:17
Annunciation, right. But you've completed it successfully
1:00:20
before and you want to get more smooth or more fluid with it, likewise for playing piano or guitar again. You have to translate to the specific cognitive and/or motor activity, that you are seeking to improve that. But
1:00:35
those at pox lasting
1:00:36
Five to 15 seconds are really the
1:00:38
Cornerstone of an effective mental training and visualization practice. And the
1:00:42
repeated nature of it 50 to 75 repetitions in. A given session
1:00:46
is also another Cornerstone of an effective mental training and visualization practice. So says this review and some of the other papers that I'm going to get to in a few moments.
1:00:55
Now, one of the other key components of a
1:00:58
successful mental training in visualization, practice is how often you perform that mental training and
1:01:04
visualization practice and
1:01:06
Net number of different Studies have looked at this
1:01:09
through a number of different lenses.
1:01:10
Meaning anywhere from two to eight times per week, it does appear that performing these sessions anywhere from three to five times
1:01:20
per week is going to be effective. We could
1:01:23
perhaps even say most effective because most of the,
1:01:25
let's just call it
1:01:27
the strongest data really point to repeating these 50 to 75 Trials, of the
1:01:32
same thing, three to five times per week so you can come up with a number that's
1:01:36
Anibal for you to do
1:01:37
consistently and you might ask. Do you have to continue to
1:01:42
perform the mental training and visualization forever? And the good news is the answer to that. Question is no, it does seem that. Once you have what's called Consolidated, the Motor Performance, or the cognitive performance of something, it can
1:01:56
be further supported or reinforced.
1:01:59
That is Consolidated in the neural circuits that are responsible for performing that mental or physical
1:02:04
task. So, in other words, once you are performing,
1:02:06
Warming that cognitive or motor tasks in a way that's
1:02:10
satisfactory or perhaps just improved. Perhaps, you're not 100%, but it's improved in the real
1:02:15
world. You don't need to continue to do mental training and visualization to to maintain
1:02:21
that real-world performance. So that's a good thing. In fact, the ideal situation would be then to pick a different sequence or thing that you're trying to learn and do mental training and visualization for that. I perhaps might have misspoke there although I don't want to edit this out.
1:02:36
Misspoke in a sense that again, I said for the thing that you're trying to learn remember, mental training, visualization is going to be most effective for building up the number of accurate
1:02:48
trials or that your ability to do something
1:02:51
with a greater frequency of something that you're
1:02:53
already capable of doing or have done it at least once in the real world. Okay.
1:02:59
This is not to say that mental training and visualization can't be used to acquire new skills. It can in principle but it
1:03:06
Been shown to be most effective for enhancing the speed and the accuracy of skills. That one has already demonstrated some degree of proficiency at in the real world. I think that's important to point out because we often
1:03:19
hear mental training, visualization, and this equivalence of perceptual and motor experiences in Our Minds Eye, to the real world and we think, oh well, we have to do is Imagine doing
1:03:26
something and we will get better at it. And unfortunately, that's not the case. The good news is however, if you can do something once even very slowly in the real world and then you bring it too.
1:03:36
The
1:03:37
mental imagery and visualization
1:03:39
domain, you can get much faster at it in a way that really does translate back to the real
1:03:42
world. I'd like to just take a brief moment and thank one of our podcast sponsors which is inside tracker inside trackers, a personalized nutrition platform. The analyzes data from your blood and DNA to help you better understand your body and help you reach your health
1:03:55
goals. I've long been a believer in getting regular blood work done for the simple reason. That blood work
1:04:01
is the only way that you can monitor the marker, such as hormone markers
1:04:04
lipids metabolic factors, Etc, the
1:04:06
Packed your immediate and long-term Health one major challenge with blood work. However,
1:04:11
is that most of the time, it does not come back with any information about what to do in order to move the values.
1:04:17
For hormones, metabolic factors, lipids Etc, into the ranges that
1:04:20
you want with inside tracker
1:04:22
changing, those values becomes very straightforward because it has a personalized dashboard that you can
1:04:26
use to address the nutrition based Behavior. Based supplement based approaches that you can use in order to move those
1:04:34
values into the ranges that are optimal for you.
1:04:36
Ooh, Your vitality and your
1:04:37
longevity inside tracker. Now includes a measurement of a pole lipoprotein be. So called a poby &, their ultimate plan. Apob is a key marker of cardiovascular
1:04:46
health and therefore there's extreme value to knowing your apob levels. If you'd like to try inside tracker, you can go to inside
1:04:52
tracker, dot, coms huberman, to get 20% off any of inside trackers plans. Again, that's inside tracker.com huberman to get 20% off. Now, if you recall principle number three or what I'm calling principle
1:05:04
number three of effective.
1:05:06
Mental training and visualization, which was that you have to be able to perform the thing that you're trying to get better at through visualization. And imagery in the real world,
1:05:17
that should raise the
1:05:17
question of what is the ratio of
1:05:20
real world training versus mental training. That's going to be most effective.
1:05:24
Well, here there are some really interesting data not just in the review that I mentioned but in a couple of the other papers that we're going to talk about in a few minutes.
1:05:31
But what I've done is I've synthesized the information across those papers and they really all point to the
1:05:36
Fact that real-world training is more effective than mental training. And mental training is more
1:05:42
effective than no training.
1:05:44
Now the mental training more effective than no training is kind of a duh except that there are people. For instance, people who are injured who are trying to maintain or replenish some motor skill or ability to move in a
1:05:57
particular way or who have had
1:06:00
traumatic brain injury and are trying to recreate experiences in a way that's safe for
1:06:05
them. Why?
1:06:06
All in a somewhat restricted format. So for instance,
1:06:09
if you've damaged a limb
1:06:11
or you're experiencing, chronic pain, and you need to take a layoff from
1:06:14
some physical activity, there are now many studies
1:06:18
looking at stroke patients at patients, that have been in accidents.
1:06:21
TBI also, people who are suffering from more conventional, Limb and connective tissue. Injuries that if they do mental training, it obviously is not going to put them at
1:06:32
risk of doing those same movements as it would in the
1:06:36
real world.
1:06:36
All right, but that it can actually accelerate or at least maintain skill performance. So it's pretty exciting if you think about it, what this means
1:06:46
and the reason it underscores this mental training is better than no training. Is
1:06:50
that should you find yourself in the unfortunate circumstance of being injured? Or unable to perform a given Behavior? Imagining the sequence of behaviour that you'd like to maintain or even build up over time. Provided you've done that
1:07:01
motor sequence before in the real
1:07:02
world. Well, the mental training visualization can really help keep that online.
1:07:06
Even help you improve over
1:07:08
time. In fact, I have a colleague in the psychology department at Stanford who told me an anecdote and I'm admittedly it's just an anecdote of a student who
1:07:17
was recruited to Stanford
1:07:19
both for their academic prowess, but also for their abilities in tennis and was injured in their first year. And at first
1:07:25
thought this was devastating. But did a cognitive reframe
1:07:29
around the idea that that
1:07:32
what's called extended lay
1:07:33
off from actual tennis
1:07:34
was going to afford them the ability to do.
1:07:36
Do more mental training than they would. Otherwise, even though
1:07:39
they were quite sad to not be able to do actual physical training for tennis
1:07:43
and when they came back from that injury, they did indeed managed to improve beyond the initial
1:07:51
non-injured State, they were in before the
1:07:53
injury, just pretty remarkable,
1:07:54
but it's called pointed out to me. They were very careful to include a lot of mental training and visualization during that quote unquote, lay off period.
1:08:02
So again, mental training better than no training physical training better than mental.
1:08:06
Inning. But when we say physical training better than mental training, what we're really talking about is when you allocate a certain amount of training hours for a
1:08:16
given skill per week, okay? So how would this look, what
1:08:19
these Studies have done is they've said, okay, if people have the option of doing the real world
1:08:24
training for 10 hours, a week versus mental training for 10 hours a week which group performs better and turns out, it's the ones that do the physical training for 10 hours per week.
1:08:33
However, we also know that combinations of
1:08:36
Physical training and mental training, can bring about results that are greater than either one of those
1:08:42
alone. How would that work? Well, I
1:08:45
wish I could tell you that if you did nine hours of physical training per week, plus 1 hour of mental training
1:08:51
that your performance will be better than if you did 10 hours of physical training and that's not the case,
1:08:57
okay? This is why we can reliably say physical real-world training
1:09:01
and again, this could be in the cognitive domain is always going to be more effective on an hour-by-hour.
1:09:06
As compared to mental training. So if you
1:09:08
can do real world training and perhaps we should be calling a real
1:09:11
world as opposed to physical. But
1:09:13
if you can do real world training compared to purely mental training, that's going to be the
1:09:19
best use of your time. This is really important, it doesn't
1:09:22
underscore everything that we're talking about because here's the really cool thing. If you do 10 hours per week of
1:09:28
real-world physical training, again, could be running, could be music, could be math,
1:09:31
could be whatever it is. You're trying to learn shooting,
1:09:34
basketballs hitting golf balls.
1:09:36
And you add one hour or
1:09:39
even half an hour of mental training to that real-world training.
1:09:45
Well, then the results are significantly greater than you would experience with physical
1:09:49
training alone and of course, it would be greater than you could achieve with mental training alone because we already established that real-world training is more powerful in learning skills and retaining skills than his mental
1:09:59
training, okay? If any of that was confusing, let me just say it one more time just to be ultra clear if you have
1:10:06
Have the option to do real world training for a
1:10:09
cognitive and/or motor skill versus
1:10:12
mental training. Always go with real-world
1:10:14
training. However, if you can add
1:10:17
to a maximum amount of
1:10:20
real-world training by doing some mental training, and you follow the principles that we've been discussing here, which are gleaned from the scientific literature.
1:10:29
Well, then you are going to get significantly greater
1:10:32
results in terms
1:10:34
of speed, accuracy and
1:10:36
consistency of performance of those real-world behaviors and cognitive abilities.
1:10:43
And, of course, if you are unable to do physical training, for whatever, reason,
1:10:48
injury travel,
1:10:50
whatever the case may be, well, then doing mental training is still
1:10:55
far significantly greater than doing no training at all.
1:10:59
Okay, so total layoffs, it turns out are a bad thing if you want to get better at something and indeed if you want to retain certain skills, both cognitive and motor.
1:11:09
Now, a couple of other things to keep in mind as you're thinking about how to build up skills through a
1:11:14
combination of physical and mental training. Well, remember, back to the beginning of the episode where we talked about neuroplasticity, and the fact that self-directed adaptive plasticity, which is really what we're talking about here in this entire episode things that you're trying to learn in a
1:11:29
A deliberate way. That is as you recall a two-part process requires
1:11:33
focused attention both when you're doing it in the real world and when you're doing mental training and it requires, rest and sleep. And in fact,
1:11:43
You would be very wise to try and get a good night's sleep. Both on the days
1:11:48
when you do physical training.
1:11:50
Again also called real world training and mental training. You
1:11:54
may also be asking, can you do them on the
1:11:56
same day
1:11:57
and this gets into some Nuance in the literature but by my reading of the literature, here's the takeaway if you are doing the maximum amount of physical training that you can do according to your schedule preventing injury
1:12:10
and all those sorts of important constraints. And
1:12:13
you're
1:12:13
Going to add mental training and imagery, it doesn't really matter
1:12:16
when you do it, you could do it
1:12:18
immediately. After your physical training, you could do it on a separate day but you do want to place it at a time in which you can try and get good sleep that night. So for instance, believe or not Studies have been done. Where people are doing mental training at
1:12:31
times when they should be
1:12:31
sleeping, that is going to offset some of the degradation in performance that you would normally see, but it's generally a bad idea. You should do your real-world training and your mental training.
1:12:43
I never it is that you can and then you should try and get as much quality sleep as you possibly
1:12:48
can on the night. Following that physical and/or, mental training, okay? This is true of pretty much every night of your life, right? If I had my way that is if I had a magic wand which obviously, I don't, I would ensure the I and everyone else in the world get sufficient amounts of quality sleep every single night. But that's just not realistic. They're going to be times where that's simply not going to happen, for whatever reason. And I always say if you're not going to get sufficient amounts of quality sleep for whatever reason, trying to make it four,
1:13:13
Fun reason or a good
1:13:14
reason but I think getting sufficient amounts of
1:13:16
quality sleep 80%, the nights of your life is a reasonable goal and one that's worth striving toward and we have lots of episodes now or three really on mastering sleep on, perfecting your sleep and episode guest episode with the great, Matthew Walker, who wrote the book, why you, why we sleep
1:13:35
incredibly important book, all of those as well as our toolkit for Sleep. Describe ways to
1:13:39
improve your sleep, so you can refer to those episodes. If you're having challenges with sleep and want to
1:13:43
Yvonne sleep and things like non sleep deep breaths, which can support your ability to sleep and your ability to learn.
1:13:48
So sleep is still vitally important. Not just, for ensuring neuroplasticity occurs following real world
1:13:53
training, but also following mental training. And again, when you place that mental training is not so critical, at least it doesn't appear to be based on the literature. So
1:14:02
if anyone out there has knowledge of any
1:14:04
peer reviewed studies stating that mental training should be done either before, or after, or some hours away from Real World Training,
1:14:11
please send that to me or put it
1:14:12
in the show.
1:14:13
Excuse me, put it in the comments on YouTube and I'll see it there because I do read all the comments but I'm not aware of any any such data or
1:14:20
analysis. And by the way, if you are interested in understanding the relationship between motor skill acquisition and retention. And this first night phenomenon of sleep, the first night, after training
1:14:30
versus sleep on the second, I
1:14:31
etcetera. There's a really wonderful paper that was published
1:14:34
by none other than the great Matthew Walker. When I believe he was a graduate student, maybe he was a postdoc when he did this in Robert stickgold slab at
1:14:41
Harvard. The title of the paper is
1:14:43
Deep and time course of motor skill
1:14:45
learning. This is a paper published. In 2003 still an incredibly important paper. I will provide a link to it in the show notes. Captions, it really highlights some of the key
1:14:54
aspects of when people sleep and how critical
1:14:57
sleep is on the night following in the nights, following that
1:15:01
training in order to really
1:15:03
consolidate certain types of learning and what phases of sleep relate to the consolidation of motor learning, Etc, a really wonderful paper and of course but just one of
1:15:13
Of Matthew and Robert stickgold incredible papers on sleep and
1:15:17
learning. Remember the beginning of the episode? When I mention that many people are good at mental training, visualization,
1:15:24
but some people are not well sex.
1:15:27
Differences, have been explored and
1:15:29
age-related. Differences have been explored in terms of people's ability to mentally visualize and train up, specific skills. And
1:15:37
while initially, there were some sex differences, identified really the bulk of the subsequent.
1:15:43
Archer.
1:15:44
That is the majority of quality peer reviewed studies on this aspect of mental training. Visualization point to the fact that there are no significant differences between males and females in terms of their ability, to mentally
1:15:54
visualize nor their ability to use that
1:15:57
mental visualization toward improving cognitive or motor skills. That point was covered in some detail in the review. I mentioned earlier, best practice for motor imagery a systematic literature. Review on motor energy training, elements in five different disciplines.
1:16:10
This review also looked at age related effects
1:16:13
And perhaps, the only thing that really popped out from this literature review, in terms of age-dependent differences that point to changes in protocols, that you might make, is that for individuals, 65 or older, a combination of physical and mental training, may actually allow them
1:16:29
to
1:16:30
gain and consolidate skills,
1:16:32
better than were they to do physical training
1:16:34
alone, now whether or not that's due to some lower upper limit of physical training that they can do because of their age, or whether or not
1:16:41
that something specific to do with.
1:16:43
Older versus younger neural circuits as in clear. But what this review also makes clear is that for the vast majority of people out
1:16:50
there, so teens people in their 20s and their 40s and so on
1:16:55
physical training, more effective, the mental training, we said that before combination of physical and mental training, more effective than physical training alone. Provided the mental training is on top of the
1:17:04
maximum amount of physical training that one could do. And of course, mental training, more effective than no training at
1:17:09
all, okay? So we talked about sets and
1:17:11
Reps, we talked about, you know,
1:17:13
5 to 15 seconds at pox with about fifteen second breaks in between or rest between sets if you will. Repeat it for 50 to 75 trials, done. Three to five times per
1:17:23
week.
1:17:25
Some of the conditions of keeping it really simple, The Importance of Being able to actually perform those sequences in
1:17:30
the real world and so on. But we haven't discussed is first person versus third
1:17:35
person and eyes open versus eyes closed. What are we really talking about here? Well,
1:17:40
first person mental training and visualization,
1:17:43
Beware. You are
1:17:45
imagining doing something
1:17:47
and you are seeing yourself doing something from the inside out as opposed from the outside in
1:17:53
Imagine, for instance wearing a head cam. Okay, we're a
1:17:55
body Cam and doing something with your hands or
1:17:58
being in virtual reality and having the sense that whatever you see in front of
1:18:02
you and that's moving and that you're doing. That's you. So what I mean by this is a mental training or visualization protocol, for instance if you were at the piano or a guitar where you're actually looking
1:18:12
down at
1:18:13
Or sensing the feeling of your hands, but you're not actually
1:18:17
moving your hands,
1:18:18
okay, as opposed to seeing
1:18:20
yourself from outside of your body. So looking at yourself, say standing next to you or from
1:18:26
across the room, you're looking at yourself playing the
1:18:28
piano or playing guitar or swinging a golf club or doing a tennis serve. Okay. First person versus third person
1:18:36
and what the data tell us is that first person mental training, visualization is generally more
1:18:41
effective than third
1:18:43
Sun' mental training visualization, which perhaps raises another chorus of does out there
1:18:48
but it needn't have been the case, right? I mean, you could imagine that seeing yourself
1:18:53
doing something and doing it perfectly because you've done it perfectly once before. Hopefully
1:18:58
would allow you to build up that skill more quickly because you have that third person perspective, where you can really see every
1:19:04
aspect in every element of what you're trying to perform.
1:19:08
Well, turns out that the first person mental training of
1:19:11
visualization is significantly more effective than that. Third person mental training and visualization. So
1:19:17
if what you're trying to learn lends itself, well to this
1:19:20
first-person mental experiencing of self as you perform the cognitive and/or, motor skill. I suggest you do that as opposed to the third person version.
1:19:30
Now what if what you're trying to learn doesn't lend
1:19:31
itself? Well to first person, visualization,
1:19:35
for instance, what if you're trying to learn specific
1:19:38
Cognitive skill, that doesn't involve
1:19:39
any overt motor Behavior to be observed.
1:19:43
Well, in that case, it's very clear
1:19:45
that closing your
1:19:47
eyes, ideally, and trying to perform that specific cognitive task or
1:19:52
the statement or the
1:19:54
uttering of a particular sentence in another language, or doing some sort of computation or problem-solving of some sort in your head. Well, that
1:20:04
itself, of course, is first person because it's inside your own body
1:20:07
as opposed to
1:20:08
I don't know that anyone would actually do this,
1:20:09
but looking at yourself from a third person perspective in your mind's eye and seeing yourself perform that cognitive challenge, whatever that challenge may happen to be.
1:20:19
Okay. Now we have to address eyes,
1:20:21
open versus eyes closed and
1:20:23
this is where the literature gets pretty interesting. I always
1:20:25
thought for some reason, I don't know why,
1:20:28
but I presumed that mental training and
1:20:30
visualization should always be done eyes closed,
1:20:34
but it turns out that's not how a lot of studies of mental training in visualization have been done. In fact,
1:20:38
Any of them have arrived at really impressive
1:20:40
protocols, which are essentially the protocols that I've distilled out in him, listing out during today's
1:20:45
episode having people, either watch videos of themselves performing a given skill and imagining themselves in that role and again, it's them. So, again, during the mental training visualization, there watching a movie of themselves, so they're somewhat in the
1:21:02
third person perspective, I guess we could technically, say they are in the third person perspective, but they're watching themselves.
1:21:08
Off. So in doing that we know based on neuroimaging studies that when we watch
1:21:12
videos of ourselves doing things, we
1:21:14
experience that more from a first-person
1:21:17
perspective. Than if we watch videos of other people doing, things use your imagination here, folks.
1:21:22
So if you're somebody for instance, who's trying to get better at a
1:21:25
particular skill, this could be not just sport, but also public speaking
1:21:29
watching videos of yourself doing that can be very effective. But of course, we have to come back to the first principle of effective mental training and visualization.
1:21:38
Which is that whatever it is that we're trying to build up or consolidate as a skill needs to be brief and repeated. So what we're really talking about here is watching a video of ourselves on Loop or listening to a audio or audio/video recording of
1:21:53
ourselves on Loop for whatever aspect that we're trying to build up or improve
1:21:57
upon. Now for people that for instance, are trying to get better at
1:22:00
dealing with public
1:22:01
speaking. And there isn't a particular skill or utterance of particular.
1:22:08
Sentences or words that they're trying to accomplish. But rather they're trying to learn to be more relaxed or to articulate better in the public. Speaking
1:22:15
scenario. There would be one of the few instances in which I suggest more General theme. And
1:22:21
not exact recapitulation of some specific words that you're going to say.
1:22:25
Perhaps it could be a sequence of you walking out onto stage toward the podium or out from the podium and facing the audience and looking in
1:22:34
multiple directions up and down to see people in every corner of the room.
1:22:38
Just repeating that on Loop in your mind's eye or watching yourself. Do that on video and making yourself calm in your internal State. As you're doing
1:22:46
that this is more of a mental autonomic training because what you're
1:22:50
really trying to do is control your autonomic nervous system. The nervous system aspect that controls how
1:22:54
alert or calm you are as opposed to a specific
1:22:57
skill. However you could also translate this to dance steps or two motor sequences for playing an
1:23:03
instrument and so on. So the point here is that
1:23:08
It's not as if there is zero utility
1:23:11
to third person mental training and visualization.
1:23:14
There can be. But first person mental training, visualization is going
1:23:20
to be more effective as I mentioned before.
1:23:21
And if you are going to use the third person, mental training, visualization, ideally you would be looking at yourself either on video or listening to yourself and audio and/or video.
1:23:34
That is going to be more effective than closing your eyes and trying to imagine
1:23:38
yourself from a third person perspective in your mind's eye.
1:23:41
Okay? So, just to make it really simple. First
1:23:43
person better than third person visualization, if you're going to go with third person, visualization, try and go with real third person, visualization where you're actually seeing and or hearing yourself on a screen. And again, this was somewhat of
1:23:54
a surprise to me. I always thought that mental
1:23:55
training, a visualization was done with eyes closed, I thought, okay, close your eyes. You imagine this, you imagine that that's actually not the case for many, many studies, some of which,
1:24:04
Are considered real, Hallmark studies within the field of mental training and visualization and the different neural circuits that it recruits and
1:24:12
along those lines. There's a really interesting study that came out, not that long
1:24:16
ago. This was just a summer of 2022 like to discuss in a little bit of detail because it really
1:24:22
hammers home a number of the principles that we talked about the title of the article is mental practice. Modulates, functional connectivity between the
1:24:28
cerebellum and the primary motor cortex
1:24:31
going to tell you the essential features of this study. First of
1:24:34
Primary motor cortex
1:24:36
sometimes called M1 is a
1:24:39
relatively small but vitally important strip of neurons in or near the front of your brain. The neurons there are called upper motor neurons. They communicate through a set of neural connections with what are called
1:24:51
lower motor neurons. The lower motor
1:24:53
neurons sit What's called the
1:24:54
ventral Horn of the spinal cord. So along the spinal cord. You have
1:24:58
sensory inputs coming from
1:24:59
skin and muscle and it's called proprioceptive feedback. That tells you where your limbs are in relation to each other.
1:25:04
And to
1:25:04
yourself and so on. You also have motor neurons that live in the spinal cord. They're actually the ones that send
1:25:09
little wires that we call axons out to the muscles. Release, acetylcholine on to those muscles and allow those muscles to
1:25:13
contract lower motor neurons are the ones that actually
1:25:17
generate movement. However,
1:25:20
they are largely responsible for reflexive. Movements are already learned movements and they require some input
1:25:26
from things like Central pattern, generators and some other circuits within the spinal cord and
1:25:29
brain stem. But it's those M1 primary motor cortex neurons that are
1:25:34
Upper motor neurons because they control lower motor neurons
1:25:38
through directed action. Okay,
1:25:41
so when I say primary motor
1:25:43
cortex, I'm really talking about those upper motor neurons M1.
1:25:46
The cerebellum is an area in the back of your brain.
1:25:49
If you were to look at a brain, you see two lobes back there that are highly foliated foliated means that lots of
1:25:53
lots of folds and lots of bumps and grooves back there. And actually means mini
1:25:57
brain. It looks like a kind of a mini brain. Stuffed in the back of the brain
1:26:01
and certain animals. The cerebellum is
1:26:03
much larger.
1:26:04
ER, than the rest of the brain in humans. The cerebellum is relatively small compared to the rest of so-called Neo cortex, the outer shell, the human brain,
1:26:11
the cerebellum is involved in Balance. It's also involved in eye movements. It's also involved in timing and motor learning, and the key thing to understand is that the cerebellum communicates with the primary motor cortex. And it can do so through what's called inhibition, it has outputs that inhibit the activity of neurons in the motor
1:26:31
cortex and elsewhere and that has a profound influence.
1:26:34
On the execution of motor Behavior and the learning of particular motor behaviors. Now, I
1:26:38
don't want to get into too much
1:26:40
detail around all this.
1:26:41
But what you need to know is that the cerebellum communicates with M1 primary motor cortex, and one is primary motor cortex. As the upper motor neurons that
1:26:49
are going to control the lower motor neurons and are going to control physical behavior and execution of physical movements.
1:26:58
The communication between cerebellum and primary motor cortex is inhibitory, although it can activate motor cortex to and this gets into a little bit of technical detail but there can be inhibition of inhibition. So if you take something that's a break and you inhibit that break what you end up with is more excitation. Okay? So the takeaway here that's key and everyone should be able to understand even though you may
1:27:19
or may not be following. This all cerebellum primary motor
1:27:22
cortex thing is that when we gain a new skill or we get more proficient at a skill,
1:27:28
So faster and more accurate. There tends to be more net, excitation of the cerebellum to
1:27:36
motor cortex, communication and that is accomplished by reducing inhibition. So that's where it gets a little bit confusing to some but
1:27:45
in this paper what they did is they explored people's ability to
1:27:48
improve on a very specific but very simple motor sequence. It's one that you're already familiar with its that tapping sequence. I talked about before where the thumb is digit, 1
1:27:57
index, finger number
1:27:58
Middle finger number three
1:28:00
ring, finger number 4 and pinky finger number 5 and it's a
1:28:03
12 13, 14, 15
1:28:05
12, 13, 14 15. And they had people actually
1:28:09
perform this and they measured their
1:28:11
speed and accuracy.
1:28:12
And then they had them do a practice session that was either an intentional task to one group, just looked at an attentional q and had to maintain focus on that attention On Cue and another group did mental practice. They basically did 50 imagine.
1:28:28
And trial. So just in their
1:28:29
minds eye of this 12, 13, 14, 15 on, repeat, okay, 50 trials, much in the same way as what, I referenced, as the ideal protocol earlier. Okay,
1:28:40
50 rounds of that, then they got tested again on the motor tasks, in the real world. And there were also recordings of the cerebellar to primary motor
1:28:50
cortex communication.
1:28:51
So, there were a bunch of different results in this study, I think are interesting. But the ones that are most important are that quote, we found
1:28:58
That mental practice enhance, both the speed and accuracy
1:29:01
of this
1:29:03
12 13, 14
1:29:04
15 performance in the real world when people did these 50. Imagine trials,
1:29:09
there are many results out there, different papers that
1:29:13
parallel. And essentially say the same thing as what is said in this paper, remember there been studies of mental training, dating back to the 1880s.
1:29:21
But what this paper really does, it looks at the neural machinery and the changes in the neural machinery and what they found using transcranial, magnetic
1:29:29
stimulation both in the context of
1:29:32
stimulating, but also recording
1:29:33
activity and connectivity between cerebellum and primary motor cortex,
1:29:37
is that mental training, enhanced the net
1:29:41
excitation of cerebellum to motor cortex communication. That is it reduced the inhibition in a way that allowed motor cortex to generate these.
1:29:51
That's with more accuracy and more speed.
1:29:53
What's also interesting about this paper is that it showed that the Improvement in performance of this task was not related to activation of the motor pathways themselves. So it's not the case that the cerebellum activation or inhibition change the patterns of excitation going directly to the spinal cord because those Pathways actually exist through a couple of intermediate stations. What it really showed is that when people do mental training, and here you could say, Okay 50, trials out
1:30:21
Out of Trials, but it's not actually that many trials, it's pretty fast learning. If you think about do a task, in the real world, do 50, Trials of the Imagine task, do the trial in the real world. Again, significant Improvement in speed and accuracy through. Now, what are becoming to be established neural circuit connections between cerebellum and primary motor
1:30:41
cortex? Okay, so this study is one of several but not a tremendous number of studies out there that are starting to really pinpoint. The underlying neural circuits
1:30:49
that allow mental training of visualization to
1:30:51
We improve motor skill performance
1:30:53
but again, and please hear me on this
1:30:56
in this study and in the
1:30:57
vast majority of other studies that have shown significant Improvement in Motor Performance in the real world,
1:31:03
by use of mental training
1:31:04
visualization. There was an ability of each and everyone in the study to perform the specific motor sequence
1:31:11
in the real world that then they were able to enhance with mental training and visualization. Now, thus far
1:31:16
we've been talking mostly about performance of motor sequences and one of the things to really understand
1:31:20
about performance of
1:31:21
Or sequences both in the real world and in the Imagine context, is that it involves the doing, is what we call a go
1:31:29
action and not
1:31:31
doing certain things. What I mean by not doing
1:31:33
well for many tasks out there.
1:31:35
Even ones as simple as the 12, 13, 14, 15 tasks that we talked about a moment ago
1:31:40
there is the need not just to tap
1:31:42
those fingers in the correct sequence as quickly as possible. But also to be accurate about it to not do 13, 14, or 13 and 4 at the same time. So there's both a
1:31:51
Go component and action component. And a withhold action component,
1:31:56
and the ability to withhold action is strongly constrained
1:31:59
by the time domain. In other words, the faster that we need to
1:32:02
perform a given motor sequence, the more likely, we are to perform incorrect
1:32:07
components of the motor sequences well. Okay, so
1:32:11
one of the key things about mental training, visualization, that's really remarkable is that it can also be used and has been shown to improve, not just go
1:32:21
aspects.
1:32:21
Motor Performance and cognitive performance, but also no go aspects of Motor Performance and skill learning.
1:32:28
Now, the go/no-go thing is
1:32:31
something I've discussed before on this podcast in reference, to the so called basal ganglia, basal ganglia
1:32:36
are subcortical. So they're below that bumpy
1:32:39
surface of the human brain that were most accustomed to seeing when we look at it from the outside.
1:32:45
And the basal ganglia are strongly involved
1:32:47
in Go versus no. Go type tasks and learning.
1:32:51
Now,
1:32:53
there are only a few studies that have really looked at the learning and the Improvement of no-go components of motor learning, but these no-go
1:33:01
components are really, really important.
1:33:04
In fact, if we were to look at what's involved at Improvement in a golf swing or shooting free, throws or getting better at piano or getting better at math or language. Speaking, I think it's fair to say that at least half and probably, as much as 75% of motor learning is
1:33:21
Out, restricting,
1:33:23
inappropriate movements, or, utterances or thoughts. If what you're trying to learn is purely cognitive and I think that's an important point. That brings us back to our initial learning. When we come into this world that developmental plasticity, which, as you recall, we have a
1:33:39
lot of interconnected aspects of our brain and nervous system early in life.
1:33:44
Remember the example of the kid trying to eat and getting the spoon of food and Bowl on their head etcetera and then over time, getting more accurate at bringing food.
1:33:51
To their mouth and eating in a clean way things that most but not all people accomplish in at some point in the course of their
1:33:57
lifetime. Well, there haven't been many, but there been a few. Very interesting studies. Looking at how mental training and visualization can improve the no-go
1:34:05
aspect of motor learning. And I think
1:34:07
this is important to highlight because it really mirrors What's Done in the real world. As opposed to just the finger tapping type things which are mostly go tasks. Again there's a little bit of a no-go component there. But there are specific tasks that
1:34:20
people have developed for
1:34:21
Laboratory that really closely mimic action learning and cognitive learning in the real
1:34:26
world. And one of the
1:34:27
more important of those is What's called the stop signal task. Now the stop signal task is something that I'll explain to you. I'll also provide a link in the show notes caption so you can try it. It's actually a lot of fun to try this because it really gives you a sense of just how challenging some of these
1:34:43
laboratory tasks are, let me just describe it for a moment.
1:34:47
The stop signal task was really developed and popularized by Gordon.
1:34:51
Logan and William Cowan. Gordon. Logan is at Vanderbilt University, and has done a lot of really important work,
1:34:58
but one of the important aspects of his work is
1:35:00
looking at Motor Performance and skill acquisition
1:35:03
and the development of the stop signal task, I'll describe the stop signal task for you. Now, in Broad Contour, you or another research subject would sit in front of a
1:35:11
screen, there are two keys on that keyboard, or two keys, among the other keys on the
1:35:16
keyboard one,
1:35:17
which is designated
1:35:19
left the other, which is designated, right?
1:35:22
And then on the screen, you'll be presented for instance with a left facing or a right facing
1:35:27
Arrow. So, in the initial trial, what would happen is that Arrow would pop up on the screen and your job
1:35:31
is to press the left key.
1:35:34
When the right-facing arrow is presented. You press the right
1:35:37
key pretty straightforward. But there's a limited amount of time in which you can do this. And the idea is that you're going to need to do this within approximately 500 milliseconds of the presentation of that Arrow or
1:35:48
else, it's going to tell you that you missed that trial. Now of course,
1:35:51
If you press the wrong key, so if the arrow goes left and you press the right key, then you would be told you got that one wrong.
1:35:57
Okay, so this
1:35:59
is a reaction time test and not one that's particularly
1:36:02
novel. What's novel? And what Logan, and cow and developed was that in the stop signal, task every once in a while. Not every trial. But every once in a while that arrow is presented and then with some delay, ranging anywhere from a hundred milliseconds to maybe 350 milliseconds, there will be a
1:36:21
Circle or a red X also
1:36:24
presented, which is a stop signal. And your job is to not press the key
1:36:29
that corresponds to the direction of arrow. In
1:36:31
fact, not press any key at all.
1:36:34
Now you can imagine how if the stop signal shows up with a longer delay after the presentation of the arrow, there's a higher probability that you will have already
1:36:43
generated the key pressing movement, okay? So
1:36:46
at the link that we provide in the show, no caption, you can actually do these two tasks and what you'll find.
1:36:51
And is that you and most people will be able to do this Arrow to Reaction Time pressing of the left to right.
1:36:59
Key somewhere in the neighborhood between 300 milliseconds and maybe as long as 500 millisecond delay, you'll get an average of how quickly respond.
1:37:06
And then, of course, if you choose to, and I would hope you would choose to go on and do the stop signal task. You will be told trial by trial whether or not you are hitting the right
1:37:16
keys, because if you are, you'll be allowed to progress to the next
1:37:20
trial, or if
1:37:21
You are told to stop that is you get the stop signal and you press the key. Anyway, you'll be told that you made an error because you did not stop. Now again, with very short delays between the presentation of the arrow in the
1:37:32
stop signal. You are going to be much better at inhibiting or preventing yourself from the behavior at the no-go aspect of motor execution. That is
1:37:42
what you will find is that if the stop signal is presented, very shortly. After let's say 100
1:37:47
milliseconds, which is very, very brief amount of time
1:37:50
after the presentation of the arrow.
1:37:51
Oh, there's a good chance that you're going to be able to withhold the key pressing behavior. However, if the delay is anywhere from 200 to 350 milliseconds, after the presentation of the arrow chances are that you're going to press the button even when you shouldn't have on at least some of those trials, okay? And if you try and game the system and wait, a certain amount of time, after the presentation of each Arrow, there will also be times in which the stop signal does not appear and you fail to
1:38:19
hit the button in the appropriate amount of
1:38:21
time. So,
1:38:21
Fun little task. It doesn't cost anything or
1:38:23
it may be a couple of minutes of your time and if you do have time to go to
1:38:27
it, I think it will give you a much deeper
1:38:29
flavor for the sorts of experiments that we're talking about here. And find that these stop signals are actually pretty hard to generate when you're trying to learn some new motor behavior. And that actually illustrates a bigger Point here,
1:38:42
if today, you sense that we've been talking about studies of, you know, tapping fingers and, you know, stopping button presses and that those examples are highly artificial.
1:38:51
Shal and don't really translate to the real world. Well, keep in mind that the tasks that are used in these studies, really Target, the specific neural circuits that is the same neural
1:39:01
circuit that you would use for the performance of essentially, any motor task. Now, of course, other motor tasks like ones where you involve your feet or
1:39:08
cognitive tasks where you have to think really hard about specific information and search for that information assemble. It in particular ways, of
1:39:14
course, involve other neurons and neural circuits that we haven't discussed today, but
1:39:19
the core components of these go and no go.
1:39:21
Task or they stop signal. Task
1:39:23
really capture the core. Elements of most,
1:39:26
all of cognitive and/or, motor learning in some way, that's fundamentally important, okay? So they have real world relevance.
1:39:33
The paper that I'd like to just briefly describe to you is entitled motor imagery combined with physical training, improves response, inhibition in the stop signal
1:39:40
task, okay? So that title is a little bit wordy but now you know
1:39:44
what the stop signal task is and what this paper essentially found was that if people did
1:39:51
physical training. So the sort of experiment that I just described
1:39:54
versus mental training, where they sat eyes open and imagine their
1:39:58
responses to those arrows and stop signals, but they didn't actually generate any key presses.
1:40:03
US versus
1:40:04
a combination of the physical training.
1:40:07
So the actual pressing of the buttons are withholding pressing of the buttons. As the case may be plus mental training.
1:40:14
Over the course of about five days using the Contour described of the key principles of mental training
1:40:22
performance. We talked about, I'll get to the specifics and moment, but it really obeyed. Most all of what we've talked about, if not all of it. So repetition simple repeated over about five days and so on and
1:40:32
so forth, what they found was that the mental training and physical training group. So mental and real-world training, groups performed significantly better in the stop signal reaction time. That is they were able to with
1:40:44
Hold action when they needed to withhold action.
1:40:48
More frequently and with more accuracy, vended either the physical training or mental training, groups alone. So this actually spits in the face of what we said earlier, which is that physical training
1:40:59
is always better than mental training. And mental training is always better than no
1:41:02
training and it's important to point out here that both the physical training and the mental training groups experienced significant improvements
1:41:10
in their reaction time and accuracy at the stop signal task.
1:41:14
But in the case of this study, which is exploring the withholding of
1:41:18
Appropriate
1:41:19
behaviors
1:41:21
the combination of mental training, and physical training,
1:41:23
out performed, either physical, or mental training alone.
1:41:26
So, while earlier, we said that, if you have a certain amount of time
1:41:30
in order to train something up, physical training is always going to be better than
1:41:34
mental training. Well, here we have somewhat of an exception where if the thing you're trying to learn involves withholding mistakes as opposed to trying to generate the right behaviors per
1:41:46
se. Well then
1:41:48
You are probably better off doing a combination of mental training and physical
1:41:52
training. Let me state that a little bit differently. If you're finding that you're screwing up something, not because you can't initiate that particular motor Behavior but you're doing the wrong thing at the wrong time, you're not able to withhold a particular action. Well then in that case, mental training in combination with physical training becomes, especially important.
1:42:12
So for you coaches for you students out there, keep that in mind
1:42:16
when trying to learn how to
1:42:17
withhold.
1:42:18
Particular action sequences because they're not serving you. Well, in the real
1:42:21
world, using a combination of real-world training and physical training is actually better for
1:42:27
you on an hour per hour basis. Then is physical training alone,
1:42:31
a couple of key details about this study, should you decide to implement these protocols in this study, they did approximately 30 Trials of the thing that they were trying to get better at now? They do those in the real world. So,
1:42:44
in this case, the stop signal tasks, involve actually pressing those buttons.
1:42:48
And then
1:42:48
Had a test phase of about 144, go, trials and about 48. Stop trials, okay? So this is important, if you are a coach or your student or your just going to self-direct, this kind of
1:43:01
learning in your self-directed adaptive, plasticity,
1:43:04
it's important that you mix in both go and no go trials, okay?
1:43:08
Wasn't always the case that there was a stop signal generated.
1:43:12
The other thing that was really impressive about the study is that the change has occurred very quickly, so the training
1:43:18
Was performed five times
1:43:20
over five days. So once a day for five days again, back to this, three to five times per week, principal and
1:43:27
the improvements were really
1:43:28
significant in some cases. In fact, if you decide to produce this paper, you can go to table
1:43:35
two. You can see, you know, in some cases, a near doubling in the reduction in reaction time through a combination of mental and
1:43:43
physical training compared to physical training alone or mental training alone again.
1:43:47
However,
1:43:48
Both physical training and mental training groups alone. Saw significant improvements but the combination of mental
1:43:55
training and physical training was far greater than you saw, with either one of those alone. So that's all nicely Quantified for you in this paper.
1:44:01
So, again, I really like this paper, despite it not involving a huge number of subjects. I think it is a key paper because it really points to such an important element of motor learning and training, which is this
1:44:14
action withholding component. This no-go component that here is captured so nice.
1:44:18
SLI in the stop signal
1:44:19
task. So before we round up our discussion about motor training of visualization want to just briefly touch on some of the studies that have explored, why certain individuals are better or worse at
1:44:31
motor training and visualization and what that might correlate with.
1:44:34
At the beginning of today's episode, I briefly mentioned a Fantasia which is
1:44:38
this phenomenon where some people just simply can't work seem to have extreme Challenge generating visual imagery.
1:44:46
Been a number of studies exploring. How a fan
1:44:48
Shakes as they're sometimes called. Although,
1:44:51
nowadays, it's not considered
1:44:53
polite. If you will, to refer to people according to their condition. So for instance, professed agnosia is a condition in which people are unable to
1:45:03
recognize particular phases.
1:45:06
And in the past, these people were referred to, as professed diagnosis X, okay, as if their condition defined them, right
1:45:14
now, it is, it's
1:45:15
not considered polite to do that, rather we say that,
1:45:18
Person has professed like no CEO or suffers from profiles like no shh, although the word suffer, then also has become a little bit touchy.
1:45:25
I'm going to do my best to just try me as clear as possible here and explain that people who have a Fantasia can have a Fantasia to varying degrees,
1:45:33
so they can either have a
1:45:34
complete absence of ability to generate mental imagery or they have a poor or kind of rudimentary ability to generate visual imagery in their minds
1:45:44
eye.
1:45:46
It was thought that people who have a Fantasia or not capable of what's called synesthesia, synesthesia has or when people have perceptual blending
1:45:56
and this is not while under the influence of any kind of psychedelic or other kind of drug
1:46:01
perceptual, blending of an
1:46:02
atypical kind or rare kind actually have some friends to friends that have different forms of synesthesia One, Associates different keys on the piano or musical notes with specific colors in a very very 12.
1:46:16
Specific way. So they'll tell you that E
1:46:17
flat on the piano is a particular tone in their mind of Amber
1:46:23
Hue. Okay. And that,
1:46:26
I forget what other key is associated with a
1:46:28
particular shade of red and so on and so forth. Are these people better at piano? Are they more
1:46:35
perceptive of colors in their environment? Not necessarily. So this is just the
1:46:38
perceptual blending, it doesn't necessarily lend itself to any improved
1:46:42
ability. Now you could imagine why
1:46:44
people would hypothesize
1:46:46
Sighs that people have a Fantasia, especially its extreme
1:46:49
form would not be capable
1:46:51
of or have synesthesia has
1:46:54
that turns out that's not the case. A couple of really interesting papers again,
1:46:58
we will link these in the show. No captions
1:47:01
one is entitled, what is the relationship between F and Tasia? Synesthesia and autism. And the other one is a Fantasia, the science of visual
1:47:09
imagery extremes. And I really like to
1:47:12
review a Fantasia, the science of visual imagery extremes for those of you that are interested,
1:47:16
In understanding a Fantasia with more
1:47:17
depth, the study addressing the relationship between a Fantasia synesthesia, and autism. Found that a Fantasia is indeed linked to week visual imagery, but that a Fantasia is can also be synesthesia X and vice versa. What was also interesting about this study is the address the question of whether or not people who have a Fantasia that is a challenge or inability to generate mental or visual imagery tend to have.
1:47:46
Features associated with autism, were residing, somewhere on the autism
1:47:49
spectrum, and I'm not trying to use ambiguous language here, but the whole
1:47:54
set of language and nomenclature around, autism, and autism spectrum is also undergoing revision now, because we are now coming to understand that autism. And nowadays, it's generally not considered correct to
1:48:07
call. People autistics in that sense, but autism
1:48:11
is considered one set of positions along a spectrum.
1:48:16
From that includes things like Asperger's Etc. But that may also include other aspects of cognition and even
1:48:22
personality. So, these are starting to be viewed, not just as a spectrum or one
1:48:26
Continuum ranging from, you know, non-autistic to autistic, but a lot of variation and subtlety in between, and even crossing over with other
1:48:35
aspects of Personality Psychology and Neuroscience. Okay, so I'm not trying to be vague here. I'm trying to be accurate rather by saying the whole
1:48:43
description and categorization of autistic
1:48:46
Non-autistic, Etc,
1:48:47
is undergoing. Vast revision right now.
1:48:49
But the important point, I think from this paper is that indeed, it was found that people who have a Fantasia tend to exhibit more of the features that are associated with
1:49:00
the autism spectrum.
1:49:01
Now, how those things relate to one another, in terms of their clinical relevance, isn't clear? And of course is entirely unclear
1:49:07
as to what's the chicken and what's the egg there.
1:49:09
So you could imagine no pun intended. For instance, that people that are on the autism spectrum might be
1:49:16
Less proficient at generating visual imagery because they are exceedingly proficient at other things. You could also imagine
1:49:25
that people are placed onto the autism spectrum. As it's sometimes referred to,
1:49:30
or are associated with particular features on the autism spectrum because in a causal way of the a Fantasia, and of course it's extremely important to highlight that, not all
1:49:40
people that
1:49:42
consider themselves or that
1:49:43
people consider Autistic or that,
1:49:46
Are on the autism spectrum or Asperger's or any variation thereof necessarily have a Fantasia.
1:49:51
Just as it is that not all people that are on the autism
1:49:55
spectrum, completely lack or even lack, what's called theory of mine, which is the ability to
1:50:00
sort of empathizing this scribe feelings and motivations
1:50:04
of others. When viewing the actions and perceived feelings of others,
1:50:09
okay? So what I just described hopefully doesn't come across as just a bunch of words soup. What I'm trying to pin point is that there does seem
1:50:16
To be a relationship between one's ability to generate visual imagery and certain constellations of cognitive and emotional
1:50:23
perception, and behavior and vice versa. Okay.
1:50:28
In a future episode, I promise to cover synesthesia and autism and some of the related cognitive and motor
1:50:36
aspects of autism. And things like, Asperger's, I'm going to feature an expert, guests or actually several expert guests in this area because it is a
1:50:43
rapidly evolving and somewhat
1:50:46
official field. Meanwhile, I think it's important to at least consider how mental training and visualization, might relate to certain aspects of cognition, and our ability to
1:50:55
visualize things not just in terms of other people's behavior, which is one of the common ways that people probe for autism and Asperger's versus non-autistic and non Asperger's. And so on the so-called theory of Mind tasks in effect, asking whether or not children are adults, can really get in the mind of others, that's a typical task developed by Simon
1:51:14
baron-cohen.
1:51:16
But also, whether or not children and adults are capable of generating mental imagery in a really Vivid way, or whether or not they have minor or
1:51:27
even extreme Challenge in doing. So,
1:51:29
and perhaps the most direct way to explain why I included this aspect of the discussion of mental training of visualization. As it relates to different cognitive phenotypes or
1:51:37
neurocognitive phenotypes such as autism, Asperger's Etc.
1:51:42
Is because if you think about motor skill,
1:51:44
execution or cognitive
1:51:46
Execution, and the relationship
1:51:47
between mental training of visualization
1:51:49
and motor skills or cognitive skills.
1:51:51
That's all pretty straightforward when you're talking about finger tapping and gonogo tasks and learning piano and things of that sort. But in many, many ways are learning of social cognition are learning of how to behave in certain circumstances, what's considered normal or a typical neurotypical and neuro, a typical if you will. A lot of that is not just generated from the inside out but it also involves observation and
1:52:15
Oh ization of what are considered appropriate and inappropriate? Definitely placed in quotes. By the way, folks, I'm
1:52:21
not placing judgment, I'm just saying
1:52:23
appropriate and inappropriate for a given
1:52:25
context behavior. In other
1:52:27
words, social learning and social cognition is every bit as much a learned behavior and pattern of cognitive and motor patterns as is tapping fingers or withholding key presses in a go no-go. Task is just that it transmits into a domain that involves
1:52:43
smiling versus frowning versus asking you.
1:52:45
Question versus staying silent versus sitting still versus fidgeting, what's
1:52:49
appropriate? And when what's inappropriate, and when all of that is what we call social cognition and
1:52:54
it has direct parallels to everything. We've been talking about up until this point. So today, we did a deep dive, which is often the case on this
1:53:01
podcast into mental training and visualization.
1:53:05
During the course of the episode. I tried to lay down one by one, the key components of an effective mental training, in
1:53:11
visualization, practice everything ranging from
1:53:13
making sure that the practice involve brief that pox repeats of specific sequences of motor and or cognitive behavior that those be relatively simple. So that you can imagine
1:53:23
them even if you're somebody who's not good at doing mental training,
1:53:26
visualization. And I should mention that if you do mental training and visualization repeatedly over time, you get better at mental training
1:53:33
and visualization, there's
1:53:35
Say What's called meta plasticity here.
1:53:36
So it's not just about engaging. Neuroplasticity of particular, circuits is also
1:53:40
about getting better at engaging plasticity. So, plasticity of plasticity,
1:53:46
also describe the key Importance of Being able to actually execute specific movements and cognitive tasks in the real world. If you want, the mental training
1:53:54
of visualization to be, especially
1:53:56
effective and we talked about the importance of naming things, we talked about
1:54:00
the importance of
1:54:01
creating, not just one, but many parallels between
1:54:05
Real
1:54:05
world training and mental training and visualization
1:54:08
and really on the whole what we established was that cognitive and/or motor learning really is something that you should do in the real world as much as possible. But if you can't do to injury or whatever conditions using mental training, is a reasonable substitute but not a complete substitute. And if you can't do a real world training for whatever reason injury, or otherwise that mental training is going to
1:54:30
be better than no training at all.
1:54:33
And of course, we established that at least for withholding action, in order to get better at a skill, a combination of physical training, and mental training is going
1:54:41
to be best. But that
1:54:43
if you're trying to learn a new skill and you're having challenges with performing that skill because of an inability to do the skill
1:54:50
in the first place, we're on a consistent
1:54:51
basis will then on an hour-by-hour basis. You're
1:54:55
best off. Investing your time into the physical training,
1:54:58
only incorporating
1:54:59
mental training and visualization. If you are able to
1:55:02
Do that, on top of the maximum amount of real-world training that you're capable of doing.
1:55:07
And of course we talked about the actual neural circuits and a bit about how
1:55:10
the actual neuroplasticity occurs
1:55:12
early in the episode. I mentioned long-term depression, well in describing the improvements in no goat a slow, stop signal tasks, a lot of what's observed during those tasks is an
1:55:22
improvement or rather an increase in long-term depression of specific neural connections.
1:55:27
So my hope is that in learning about those basic neural circuits and plasticity mechanisms,
1:55:32
And in learning about the critical importance of
1:55:34
focus and attention during learning both real world and imagined as well as the importance of sleep and deep rest for really consolidating learning and the different tools. The various steps are principles of
1:55:48
effective, mental training,
1:55:49
visualization. That you now have a fairly coherent, or maybe even a very coherent picture of how to develop the best mental training and visualization
1:55:57
protocols for you.
1:55:58
I realize that everyone has different goals. Everyone has different time, constraints.
1:56:02
If you are somebody that's interested in developing a mental training of visualization protocol. So if you're a coach or teacher, or simply a learner or you're trying to self-direct your
1:56:10
own adaptive plasticity,
1:56:12
I want to emphasize that the key components that we discussed today are essential to include but I wouldn't obsess about whether or
1:56:19
not a given epic is 15 or 20 seconds or even 25 seconds.
1:56:23
I wouldn't have assessed over whether or not you got 30 repetitions in, then your mind drifted, or whether or not
1:56:27
you could do the full 50 to
1:56:28
75 or whether or not even in your mind's eye you made some errors.
1:56:32
Hers. What's been shown over and over again? In this literature, is that performing mental training visualization repeatedly. And in a very restricted way, that makes it easier to perform those trials
1:56:44
over and over and over again and with a high degree of accuracy,
1:56:48
almost always. Really, we can fairly say in essentially every study where it's been explored has led to improvements in real world.
1:56:56
Performance of both cognitive and/or physical tasks,
1:56:59
so if you're trying to learn anything at all, I do encourage you.
1:57:02
Explorer motor training and visualization because basically all the studies out
1:57:07
there fact I couldn't find one exception, where
1:57:10
some degree of improvement, wasn't observed, when people use motor
1:57:14
training a visualization on a consistent basis, even just the three to five times per week. The simple repeats over and over. So I don't want to
1:57:20
over complicate, or make it sound like mental training and visualization has to be performed in a very precise way or that it has to be done perfectly each and every time quite to the contrary what is clear is that mental training
1:57:32
Any visualization is a very effective way to
1:57:34
improve real-world performance.
1:57:36
If you're learning from, and, or enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube
1:57:39
channel. That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
1:57:42
In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on both Spotify, and
1:57:46
apple and on both Spotify and apple. You can leave us up to a five star review
1:57:50
if you have questions for us or comments about the podcast or guess you'd like me to feature on the huberman Lab podcast. Please put those in the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments, please. Also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and
1:58:01
throughout.
1:58:02
Today's episode, that's the best way to support the huberman Lab
1:58:04
podcast. Not so much on today's episode but on many previous episodes of The
1:58:09
huberman Lab podcast, we discuss supplements, while
1:58:11
supplements are necessary for everybody. Many people drive tremendous benefit
1:58:14
from them for things like improving
1:58:16
sleep for hormone augmentation and for improving, Focus huberman. Lab podcast is proud to have partnered with Momentis supplements. If you'd like to learn more about the supplements discuss on the huberman Lab podcast. Please go to live momentous spell do you s.com huberman again? That's live.
1:58:32
S.com huberman. If you're not already following huberman lab on social media, I am huberman lab on LinkedIn, Facebook, Twitter and Instagram. And it all those places. I cover content. Some of which overlaps with the content of the huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from
1:58:46
content on the huberman Lab podcast. So
1:58:48
again, it's huberman
1:58:49
lab on all social media platforms.
1:58:51
Also, I know many of you are interested in summaries of podcasts and what we call tool kits, which describe Ideal tool kits and
1:58:59
protocols for sleeper Ideal tool, kit and protocols.
1:59:02
Or plasticity or for deliberate cold exposure, at cetera
1:59:05
for that reason, we've established What's called the neural network newsletter? This is a completely zero cost newsletter that you can sign up for by going to huberman lab.com. Go to the menu, scroll down to newsletter, and you sign up by providing your email. We do not share your email with anybody and there are also some sample PDFs of existing huberman Lab podcast. Protocols again ranging from neural
1:59:24
plasticity to sleep and other topics that we've covered in brief one to three page summaries.
1:59:29
Thank you, once again, for joining me for today's discussion.
1:59:32
All about the science and effective implementation of mental training and visualization and last, but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.
ms